Larger visual stimuli are perceived to last longer from time to time: The internal clock is not affected by nontemporal visual stimulus size.

Abstract:

:Performance on interval timing is often explained by the assumption of an internal clock based on neural counting. According to this account, a neural pacemaker generates pulses, and the number of pulses relating to a physical time interval is recorded by a counter. Thus, the number of accumulated pulses is the internal representation of this interval. Several studies demonstrated that large visual stimuli are perceived to last longer than smaller ones presented for the same duration. The present study was designed to investigate whether nontemporal visual stimulus size directly affects the internal clock. For this purpose, a temporal reproduction task was applied. Sixty participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions with stimulus size being experimentally varied within either the target or the reproduction interval. A direct effect of nontemporal stimulus size on the pacemaker-counter system should become evident irrespective of whether stimulus size was experimentally varied within the target or the reproduction interval. An effect of nontemporal stimulus size on reproduced duration only occurred when stimulus size was varied during the target interval. This finding clearly argues against the notion that nontemporal visual stimulus size directly affects the internal clock. Furthermore, our findings ruled out a decisional bias as a possible cause of the observed differential effect of stimulus size on reproduced duration. Rather the effect of stimulus size appeared to originate from the memory stage of temporal information processing at which the timing signal from the pacemaker-counter component is encoded in reference memory.

journal_name

J Vis

journal_title

Journal of vision

authors

Rammsayer TH,Verner M

doi

10.1167/15.3.5

subject

Has Abstract

pub_date

2015-03-10 00:00:00

issue

3

issn

1534-7362

pii

15.3.5

journal_volume

15

pub_type

杂志文章
  • The face-in-the-crowd effect: Threat detection versus iso-feature suppression and collinear facilitation.

    abstract::Are people biologically prepared for the rapid detection of threat posed by an angry facial expression, even when it is conveyed in the form of a schematic line drawing? Based on visual search times, the current literature would suggest that the answer is yes. But are there low-level explanations for this effect? Here...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/19.7.6

    authors: Kennett MJ,Wallis G

    更新日期:2019-07-01 00:00:00

  • Transfer in motion discrimination learning was no greater in double training than in single training.

    abstract::We investigated the controversy regarding double training in motion discrimination learning. We collected data from 43 participants in a motion direction discrimination learning task with either double training (i.e., training plus exposure) or single training (i.e., no exposure). By pooling these data with those in t...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.6.7

    authors: Huang J,Liang J,Zhou Y,Liu Z

    更新日期:2017-06-01 00:00:00

  • Visual motion, eye motion, and relative motion: A parametric fMRI study of functional specializations of smooth pursuit eye movement network areas.

    abstract::The ability to pursue moving objects with the eyes is vital to humans. However, it remains unclear how the brain differentiates visual object motion, smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM), and eye movement-induced relative motion on the retina and where visual-to-oculomotor transformation takes place. To characterize fu...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.14.21

    authors: Ohlendorf S,Sprenger A,Speck O,Glauche V,Haller S,Kimmig H

    更新日期:2010-12-20 00:00:00

  • The perception of 2D orientation is categorically biased.

    abstract::Three experimental paradigms were used to investigate the perception of orientation relative to internal categorical standards of vertical and horizontal. In Experiment 1, magnitude estimation of orientation (in degrees) relative to vertical and horizontal replicated a previously reported spatial orientation bias also...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/11.8.13

    authors: Durgin FH,Li Z

    更新日期:2011-07-22 00:00:00

  • Topographical representation of binocular depth in the human visual cortex using fMRI.

    abstract::We used binocular stimuli to define how the visual location of stereoscopic depth structure maps topographically onto the human visual cortex. The main stimulus consisted of a circular disk of dots, most at zero-disparity, against which a single quadrant was defined with changing disparity ('correlated' disparity), an...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/7.14.15

    authors: Bridge H,Parker AJ

    更新日期:2007-12-17 00:00:00

  • Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location.

    abstract::In the peripheral visual field, nearby objects can make one another difficult to recognize (crowding) in a manner that critically depends on their separation. We manipulated the apparent separation of objects using the illusory shifts in perceived location that arise from local motion to determine if crowding depends ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/11.9.2

    authors: Dakin SC,Greenwood JA,Carlson TA,Bex PJ

    更新日期:2011-08-08 00:00:00

  • Depth estimation from retinal disparity requires eye and head orientation signals.

    abstract::To reach for an object, one needs to know its egocentric distance (absolute depth). It remains an unresolved issue which signals are required by the brain to calculate this absolute depth information. We devised a geometric model of binocular 3D eye orientation and investigated the signals necessary to uniquely determ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.16.3

    authors: Blohm G,Khan AZ,Ren L,Schreiber KM,Crawford JD

    更新日期:2008-12-15 00:00:00

  • Optimal stimulus encoders for natural tasks.

    abstract::Determining the features of natural stimuli that are most useful for specific natural tasks is critical for understanding perceptual systems. A new approach is described that involves finding the optimal encoder for the natural task of interest, given a relatively small population of noisy "neurons" between the encode...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.13.17

    authors: Geisler WS,Najemnik J,Ing AD

    更新日期:2009-12-16 00:00:00

  • The face-in-the-crowd effect: when angry faces are just cross(es).

    abstract::A common theme running through much of the visual recognition literature is that faces are special. Many studies now describe evidence for the idea that faces are processed in a dedicated center in cortex. Studies have also argued for the presence of evolutionarily expedient pathways dedicated to the processing of cer...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.1.7

    authors: Coelho CM,Cloete S,Wallis G

    更新日期:2010-01-13 00:00:00

  • The segmental structure of faces and its use in gender recognition.

    abstract::What is the relationship between object segmentation and recognition? First, we develop a feature segmentation method that parses faces into features and, in doing so, attempts to approximate human performance. This segmentation-based approach allows us to build featural representations that make explicit the part-who...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.7.7

    authors: Nestor A,Tarr MJ

    更新日期:2008-05-23 00:00:00

  • Impact of astigmatism and high-order aberrations on subjective best focus.

    abstract::We studied the role of native astigmatism and ocular aberrations on best-focus setting and its shift upon induction of astigmatism in 42 subjects (emmetropes, myopes, hyperopes, with-the-rule [WTR] and against-the-rule [ATR] myopic astigmats). Stimuli were presented in a custom-developed adaptive optics simulator, all...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.11.4

    authors: Marcos S,Velasco-Ocana M,Dorronsoro C,Sawides L,Hernandez M,Marin G

    更新日期:2015-08-01 00:00:00

  • Cross-orientation masking is speed invariant between ocular pathways but speed dependent within them.

    abstract::In human (D. H. Baker, T. S. Meese, & R. J. Summers, 2007b) and in cat (B. Li, M. R. Peterson, J. K. Thompson, T. Duong, & R. D. Freeman, 2005; F. Sengpiel & V. Vorobyov, 2005) there are at least two routes to cross-orientation suppression (XOS): a broadband, non-adaptable, monocular (within-eye) pathway and a more na...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.5.2

    authors: Meese TS,Baker DH

    更新日期:2009-05-04 00:00:00

  • Serial dependence in the perception of visual variance.

    abstract::The recent history of perceptual experience has been shown to influence subsequent perception. Classically, this dependence on perceptual history has been examined in sensory-adaptation paradigms, wherein prolonged exposure to a particular stimulus (e.g., a vertically oriented grating) produces changes in perception o...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/18.7.4

    authors: Suárez-Pinilla M,Seth AK,Roseboom W

    更新日期:2018-07-02 00:00:00

  • Motion perception by a moving observer in a three-dimensional environment.

    abstract::Perceiving three-dimensional object motion while moving through the world is hard: not only must optic flow be segmented and parallax resolved into shape and motion, but also observer motion needs to be taken into account in order to perceive absolute, rather than observer-relative motion. In order to simplify the las...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/13.2.15

    authors: Dupin L,Wexler M

    更新日期:2013-02-08 00:00:00

  • Partial modal completion under occlusion: what do modal and amodal percepts represent?

    abstract::In the occlusion illusion, a partly occluded object is perceived as though it were less occluded than it actually is (Palmer, Brooks, & Lai, 2007). We confirm and extend this finding using a stimulus with a moving occluder. In agreement with Palmer et al.'s (2007) findings and their partial-modal-completion hypothesis...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.1.22

    authors: Scherzer TR,Ekroll V

    更新日期:2015-01-22 00:00:00

  • Beyond distance and direction: the brain represents target locations non-metrically.

    abstract::In their day-to-day activities human beings are constantly generating behavior, such as pointing, grasping or verbal reports, on the basis of visible target locations. The question arises how the brain represents target locations. One possibility is that the brain represents them metrically, i.e. in terms of distance ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.3.3

    authors: Thaler L,Goodale MA

    更新日期:2010-03-23 00:00:00

  • Counting on the motor system: rapid action planning reveals the format- and magnitude-dependent extraction of numerical quantity.

    abstract::Symbolic numbers (e.g., "2") acquire their meaning by becoming linked to the core nonsymbolic quantities they represent (e.g., two items). However, the extent to which symbolic and nonsymbolic information converges onto the same internal core representations of quantity remains a point of considerable debate. As nearl...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.3.30

    authors: Chapman CS,Gallivan JP,Wood DK,Milne JL,Ansari D,Culham JC,Goodale MA

    更新日期:2014-03-26 00:00:00

  • Feature integration is unaffected by saccade landing point, even when saccades land outside of the range of regular oculomotor variance.

    abstract::The experience of our visual surroundings appears continuous, contradicting the erratic nature of visual processing due to saccades. A possible way the visual system can construct a continuous experience is by integrating presaccadic and postsaccadic visual input. However, saccades rarely land exactly at the intended ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/18.7.6

    authors: Schut MJ,Van der Stoep N,Fabius JH,Van der Stigchel S

    更新日期:2018-07-02 00:00:00

  • The perception of gloss depends on highlight congruence with surface shading.

    abstract::Studies have shown that displacing specular highlights from their natural locations in images reduces perceived surface gloss. Here, we assessed the extent to which perceived gloss depends on congruence in the position and orientation of specular highlights relative to surface shape and the diffuse shading from which ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/11.9.4

    authors: Kim J,Marlow P,Anderson BL

    更新日期:2011-08-12 00:00:00

  • Differential changes in human perception of speed due to motion adaptation.

    abstract::Visual systems adapt to the prevailing image conditions. This improves the ability to discriminate between two similar stimuli but has the side effect that veridical perception is degraded. For example, prolonged driving at 100 km/h may reduce the perceived speed to 80 km/h but improve the sensitivity to changes in th...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.11.6

    authors: Hietanen MA,Crowder NA,Ibbotson MR

    更新日期:2008-08-11 00:00:00

  • Orientations for the successful categorization of facial expressions and their link with facial features.

    abstract::Horizontal information was recently suggested to be crucial for face identification. In the present paper, we expand on this finding and investigate the role of orientations for all the basic facial expressions and neutrality. To this end, we developed orientation bubbles to quantify utilization of the orientation spe...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.14.7

    authors: Duncan J,Gosselin F,Cobarro C,Dugas G,Blais C,Fiset D

    更新日期:2017-12-01 00:00:00

  • Sensory and decisional factors in motion-induced blindness.

    abstract::The processes underlying motion-induced blindness (MIB) are widely debated. Ultimately, however, they must reduce to a sensitivity drop and/or to an upward shift of the decision criterion. The first possibility was tested by assessing the detection threshold for a contrast (or luminance) increment applied to the MIB t...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/7.7.4

    authors: Caetta F,Gorea A,Bonneh Y

    更新日期:2007-05-23 00:00:00

  • Perceptual learning transfer between hemispheres and tasks for easy and hard feature search conditions.

    abstract::Perceptual learning involves modification of cortical processes so that transfer to new task variants depends on neuronal representation overlap. Neuron selectivity varies with cortical level, so that the degree of transfer should depend on training-induced modification level. We ask how different can stimuli be, how ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/11.1.8

    authors: Pavlovskaya M,Hochstein S

    更新日期:2011-01-07 00:00:00

  • Nulling the motion aftereffect with dynamic random-dot stimuli: limitations and implications.

    abstract::We used biased random-dot dynamic test stimuli to measure the strength of the motion aftereffect (MAE) to evaluate the usefulness of this technique as a measure of motion adaptation strength. The stimuli consisted of noise dots whose individual directions were random and of signal dots moving in a unique direction. Al...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/2.4.3

    authors: Castet E,Keeble DR,Verstraten FA

    更新日期:2002-01-01 00:00:00

  • Interocular suppression patterns in binocularly abnormal observers using luminance- and contrast-modulated noise stimuli.

    abstract::In binocular viewing, images presented to the amblyopic eye are suppressed in the cortex to prevent confusion or diplopia. The present study measures depth and extent of interocular suppression across the central circular 24° visual field in observers with strabismus and microstrabismus. Visual stimuli were concentric...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/16.10.20

    authors: Chima AS,Formankiewicz MA,Waugh SJ

    更新日期:2016-08-01 00:00:00

  • Selectivity and robustness of sparse coding networks.

    abstract::We investigate how the population nonlinearities resulting from lateral inhibition and thresholding in sparse coding networks influence neural response selectivity and robustness. We show that when compared to pointwise nonlinear models, such population nonlinearities improve the selectivity to a preferred stimulus an...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/jov.20.12.10

    authors: Paiton DM,Frye CG,Lundquist SY,Bowen JD,Zarcone R,Olshausen BA

    更新日期:2020-11-02 00:00:00

  • Activity in visual area V4 correlates with surface perception.

    abstract::The neural mechanisms responsible for unifying noncontiguous regions of a visual image into a percept of a single surface remain largely unknown. To investigate these mechanisms, we used a novel stimulus in which local luminance was the only cue for surface segmentation. Subjects viewed an array of small adjoining ele...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.7.28

    authors: Bouvier SE,Cardinal KS,Engel SA

    更新日期:2008-11-07 00:00:00

  • Cue combination on the circle and the sphere.

    abstract::Bayesian cue combination models have been used to examine how human observers combine information from several cues to form estimates of linear quantities like depth. Here we develop an analogous theory for circular quantities like planar direction. The circular theory is broadly similar to the linear theory but diffe...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.11.15

    authors: Murray RF,Morgenstern Y

    更新日期:2010-09-01 00:00:00

  • Effects of number, complexity, and familiarity of flankers on crowded letter identification.

    abstract::We tested identification of target letters surrounded by a varying number (2, 4, 6) of horizontally aligned flanking elements. Strings were presented left or right of a central fixation dot, and targets were always at the center of the string. Flankers could be other letters, digits, symbols, simple shapes, or false f...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.6.7

    authors: Chanceaux M,Mathôt S,Grainger J

    更新日期:2014-11-10 00:00:00

  • The reward of seeing: Different types of visual reward and their ability to modify oculomotor learning.

    abstract::Saccadic adaptation is an oculomotor learning process that maintains the accuracy of eye movements to ensure effective perception of the environment. Although saccadic adaptation is commonly considered an automatic and low-level motor calibration in the cerebellum, we recently found that strength of adaptation is infl...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.12.11

    authors: Meermeier A,Gremmler S,Richert K,Eckermann T,Lappe M

    更新日期:2017-10-01 00:00:00