Nulling the motion aftereffect with dynamic random-dot stimuli: limitations and implications.

Abstract:

:We used biased random-dot dynamic test stimuli to measure the strength of the motion aftereffect (MAE) to evaluate the usefulness of this technique as a measure of motion adaptation strength. The stimuli consisted of noise dots whose individual directions were random and of signal dots moving in a unique direction. All dots moved at the same speed. For each condition, the nulling percentage (percentage of signal dots needed to perceptually null the MAE) was scaled with respect to the coherence threshold (percentage needed to perceive the coherent motion of signal dots without prior adaptation). The increase of these scaled values with the density of dots in the test stimulus suggests that MAE strength is underestimated when measured with low densities. We show that previous reports of high nulling percentages at slow speeds do not reflect strong MAEs, but are actually due to spatio-temporal aliasing, which dramatically increases coherence thresholds. We further show that MAE strength at slow speed increases with eccentricity. These findings are consistent with the idea that using this dynamic test stimulus preferentially reveals the adaptation of a population of high-speed motion units whose activity is independent of adapted low-speed motion units.

journal_name

J Vis

journal_title

Journal of vision

authors

Castet E,Keeble DR,Verstraten FA

doi

10.1167/2.4.3

keywords:

subject

Has Abstract

pub_date

2002-01-01 00:00:00

pages

302-11

issue

4

issn

1534-7362

pii

2/4/3

journal_volume

2

pub_type

杂志文章
  • Resolution of blur in the older eye: neural compensation in addition to optics?

    abstract::This study examined the roles of pupillary miosis and experience-mediated compensation in older observers' superior ability to read optically blurred text. The size thresholds of younger and older adult observers for reading common words and identifying line drawings of everyday objects were compared with natural and ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.5.7

    authors: Jung GH,Kline DW

    更新日期:2010-05-01 00:00:00

  • Signs of depth-luminance covariance in 3-D cluttered scenes.

    abstract::In three-dimensional (3-D) cluttered scenes such as foliage, deeper surfaces often are more shadowed and hence darker, and so depth and luminance often have negative covariance. We examined whether the sign of depth-luminance covariance plays a role in depth perception in 3-D clutter. We compared scenes rendered with ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/18.3.5

    authors: Scaccia M,Langer MS

    更新日期:2018-03-01 00:00:00

  • Position shifts following crowded second-order motion adaptation reveal processing of local and global motion without awareness.

    abstract::Adaptation to first-order (luminance defined) motion produces not only a motion aftereffect but also a position aftereffect, in which a target pattern's perceived location is shifted opposite the direction of adaptation. These aftereffects can occur passively (when the direction of motion adaptation cannot be detected...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/7.2.15

    authors: Harp TD,Bressler DW,Whitney D

    更新日期:2007-07-20 00:00:00

  • Switching between visuomotor mappings: learning absolute mappings or relative shifts.

    abstract::Adaptation to specific visuomotor mappings becomes faster when switching back and forth between them. What is learned when repeatedly switching between the visuomotor mappings: the absolute mappings or the relative shift between the mappings? To test this, we trained participants in a rapid pointing task using a uniqu...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/13.2.26

    authors: van Dam LC,Hawellek DJ,Ernst MO

    更新日期:2013-01-01 00:00:00

  • Motion-induced position shift in stereoscopic and dichoptic viewing.

    abstract::The static envelope of a Gabor patch with a moving sinusoidal carrier appears shifted in the direction of the carrier motion (De Valois & De Valois, 1991). This phenomenon is called motion-induced position shift. Although several motion-processing stages, ranging from low- to high-level processes, may contribute to po...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/16.13.3

    authors: Hisakata R,Hayashi D,Murakami I

    更新日期:2016-10-01 00:00:00

  • Gaze behavior during 3-D face identification is depth cue invariant.

    abstract::Gaze behavior during scene and object recognition can highlight the relevant information for a task. For example, salience maps-highlighting regions that have heightened luminance, contrast, color, etc. in a scene-can be used to predict gaze targets. Certain tasks, such as face recognition, result in a typical pattern...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.2.9

    authors: Akhavein H,Farivar R

    更新日期:2017-02-01 00:00:00

  • Global attention facilitates the planning, but not execution of goal-directed reaches.

    abstract::In daily life, humans interact with multiple objects in complex environments. A large body of literature demonstrates that target selection is biased toward recently attended features, such that reaches are faster and trajectory curvature is reduced when target features (i.e., color) are repeated (priming of pop-out)....

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/16.9.7

    authors: McCarthy JD,Song JH

    更新日期:2016-07-01 00:00:00

  • Reaching and grasping actions and their context shape the perception of object size.

    abstract::Humans frequently estimate the size of objects to grasp them. In fact, when performing an action, our perception is focused towards the visual properties of the object that enable us to successfully execute the action. However, the motor system is also able to influence perception, but only a few studies have reported...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.12.10

    authors: Bosco A,Daniele F,Fattori P

    更新日期:2017-10-01 00:00:00

  • Interactions between luminance and color signals: effects on shape.

    abstract::Although luminance and color are thought to be processed independently at early stages of visual processing, there is evidence that they interact at later stages. For example, chromatic information has been shown to enhance or suppress depth from luminance depending on whether chromatic edges are aligned or orthogonal...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章,评审

    doi:10.1167/13.5.16

    authors: Clery S,Bloj M,Harris JM

    更新日期:2013-04-18 00:00:00

  • Interocular suppression patterns in binocularly abnormal observers using luminance- and contrast-modulated noise stimuli.

    abstract::In binocular viewing, images presented to the amblyopic eye are suppressed in the cortex to prevent confusion or diplopia. The present study measures depth and extent of interocular suppression across the central circular 24° visual field in observers with strabismus and microstrabismus. Visual stimuli were concentric...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/16.10.20

    authors: Chima AS,Formankiewicz MA,Waugh SJ

    更新日期:2016-08-01 00:00:00

  • Accuracy and precision of objective refraction from wavefront aberrations.

    abstract::We determined the accuracy and precision of 33 objective methods for predicting the results of conventional, sphero-cylindrical refraction from wavefront aberrations in a large population of 200 eyes. Accuracy for predicting defocus (as specified by the population mean error of prediction) varied from -0.50 D to +0.25...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/4.4.9

    authors: Thibos LN,Hong X,Bradley A,Applegate RA

    更新日期:2004-04-23 00:00:00

  • Visual working memory for briefly presented scenes.

    abstract::Previous studies have painted a conflicting picture on the amount of visual information humans can extract from viewing a natural scene briefly. Although some studies suggest that a single glimpse is sufficient to put about five visual objects in memory, others find that not much is retained in visual memory even afte...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/5.7.5

    authors: Liu K,Jiang Y

    更新日期:2005-09-16 00:00:00

  • Estimating nonlinear receptive fields from natural images.

    abstract::The response of visual cells is a nonlinear function of their stimuli. In addition, an increasing amount of evidence shows that visual cells are optimized to process natural images. Hence, finding good nonlinear models to characterize visual cells using natural stimuli is important. The Volterra model is an appealing ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/6.4.11

    authors: Rapela J,Mendel JM,Grzywacz NM

    更新日期:2006-05-16 00:00:00

  • Pupil size is modulated by the size of equal-luminance gratings.

    abstract::Pupil size changes with light. For this reason, researchers studying the effect of attention, contextual processing, and arousal on the pupillary response have matched the mean luminance of their stimuli across conditions to eliminate the contribution of differences in light levels. Here, we argue that the match of me...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/jov.20.8.4

    authors: Gao J,Ko A,Yabe Y,Goodale MA,Chen J

    更新日期:2020-08-03 00:00:00

  • Gradients of relative disparity underlie the perceived slant of stereoscopic surfaces.

    abstract::Perceived stereoscopic slant around a vertical axis is strongly underestimated for isolated surfaces, suggesting that neither uniocular image compression nor linear gradients of absolute disparity are very effective cues. However, slant increases to a level close to geometric prediction if gradients of relative dispar...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/16.5.16

    authors: Wardle SG,Gillam BJ

    更新日期:2016-01-01 00:00:00

  • The time course of the inversion effect during individual face discrimination.

    abstract::Human faces look more similar to each other when they are presented upside down, leading to an increase in error rate and response time during individual face discrimination tasks. This face inversion effect (FIE) is one of the most robust findings in the face processing literature. Recent neuroimaging studies using a...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/7.8.3

    authors: Jacques C,d'Arripe O,Rossion B

    更新日期:2007-06-08 00:00:00

  • Roles of visual and non-visual information in the perception of scene-relative object motion during walking.

    abstract::Perceiving object motion during self-movement is an essential ability of humans. Previous studies have reported that the visual system can use both visual information (such as optic flow) and non-visual information (such as vestibular, somatosensory, and proprioceptive information) to identify and globally subtract th...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/jov.20.10.15

    authors: Xie M,Niehorster DC,Lappe M,Li L

    更新日期:2020-10-01 00:00:00

  • Uncovering gender discrimination cues in a realistic setting.

    abstract::Which face cues do we use for gender discrimination? Few studies have tried to answer this question and the few that have tried typically used only a small set of grayscale stimuli, often distorted and presented a large number of times. Here, we reassessed the importance of facial cues for gender discrimination in a m...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.2.10

    authors: Dupuis-Roy N,Fortin I,Fiset D,Gosselin F

    更新日期:2009-02-10 00:00:00

  • Different aberrations raise contrast thresholds for single-letter identification in line with their effect on cross-correlation-based confusability.

    abstract::We previously showed that different types of aberration defocus, coma, and secondary astigmatism affect reading performance via different mechanisms. In this paper, we show the contrary result that, for identification of isolated letters, the effects of rendering different types of aberration can be described by a sin...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/13.7.12

    authors: Young LK,Love GD,Smithson HE

    更新日期:2013-06-20 00:00:00

  • Salience of the lambs: a test of the saliency map hypothesis with pictures of emotive objects.

    abstract::Humans have an ability to rapidly detect emotive stimuli. However, many emotional objects in a scene are also highly visually salient, which raises the question of how dependent the effects of emotionality are on visual saliency and whether the presence of an emotional object changes the power of a more visually salie...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/12.1.22

    authors: Humphrey K,Underwood G,Lambert T

    更新日期:2012-01-25 00:00:00

  • Independent anatomical and functional measures of the V1/V2 boundary in human visual cortex.

    abstract::The cerebral cortex has both anatomical and functional specialization, but the level of correspondence between the two in the human brain has remained largely elusive. Recent successes in high-resolution magnetic resonance imaging of myeloarchitecture patterns in the cortex suggest that it may now be possible to compa...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/5.2.1

    authors: Bridge H,Clare S,Jenkinson M,Jezzard P,Parker AJ,Matthews PM

    更新日期:2005-02-11 00:00:00

  • A horizontal bias in human visual processing of orientation and its correspondence to the structural components of natural scenes.

    abstract::Many encoding mechanisms and processing strategies in the visual system appear to have evolved to better process the prevalent content in the visual world. Here we examine the relationship between the prevalence of natural scene content at different orientations and visual ability for detecting oriented natural scene ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/4.12.5

    authors: Hansen BC,Essock EA

    更新日期:2004-12-10 00:00:00

  • The Bouma law of crowding, revised: critical spacing is equal across parts, not objects.

    abstract::Crowding is the inability to identify an object among flankers in the periphery. It is due to inappropriate incorporation of features from flanking objects in perception of the target. Crowding is characterized by measuring critical spacing, the minimum distance needed between a target and flankers to allow recognitio...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.6.10

    authors: Rosen S,Chakravarthi R,Pelli DG

    更新日期:2014-12-10 00:00:00

  • Learning Bayesian priors for depth perception.

    abstract::How the visual system learns the statistical regularities (e.g., symmetry) needed to interpret pictorial cues to depth is one of the outstanding questions in perceptual science. We test the hypothesis that the visual system can adapt its model of the statistics of planar figures for estimating three-dimensional surfac...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/7.8.13

    authors: Knill DC

    更新日期:2007-06-26 00:00:00

  • Anatomic and functional asymmetries interactively shape human early visual cortex responses.

    abstract::Early visual processing is surprisingly flexible even in the adult brain. This flexibility involves both long-term structural plasticity and online adaptations conveyed by top-down feedback. Although this view is supported by rich evidence from both human behavioral studies and invasive electrophysiology in nonhuman m...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/jov.20.6.3

    authors: Herde L,Uhl J,Rauss K

    更新日期:2020-06-03 00:00:00

  • A moving-barber-pole illusion.

    abstract::In the barber-pole illusion (BPI), a diagonally moving grating is perceived as moving vertically because of the shape of the vertically oriented window through which it is viewed-a strong shape-motion interaction. We introduce a novel stimulus-the moving barber pole-in which a diagonal, drifting sinusoidal carrier is ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.5.1

    authors: Sun P,Chubb C,Sperling G

    更新日期:2014-05-01 00:00:00

  • What limits performance in the amblyopic visual system: seeing signals in noise with an amblyopic brain.

    abstract::Amblyopia results in a loss of visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and position acuity. However, the nature of the neural losses is not yet fully understood. Here we report the results of experiments using noise to try to better understand the losses in amblyopia. Specifically, in one experiment we compared the perfo...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.4.1

    authors: Levi DM,Klein SA,Chen I

    更新日期:2008-04-04 00:00:00

  • Transfer in motion discrimination learning was no greater in double training than in single training.

    abstract::We investigated the controversy regarding double training in motion discrimination learning. We collected data from 43 participants in a motion direction discrimination learning task with either double training (i.e., training plus exposure) or single training (i.e., no exposure). By pooling these data with those in t...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.6.7

    authors: Huang J,Liang J,Zhou Y,Liu Z

    更新日期:2017-06-01 00:00:00

  • Is the ability to identify deviations in multiple trajectories compromised by amblyopia?

    abstract::Amblyopia results in a severe loss of positional information and in the ability to accurately enumerate objects (V. Sharma, D. M. Levi, & S. A. Klein, 2000). In this study, we asked whether amblyopia also disrupts the ability to track a near-threshold change in the trajectory of a single target amongst multiple simila...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/6.12.3

    authors: Levi DM,Tripathy SP

    更新日期:2006-11-28 00:00:00

  • Synchronous motion modulates animacy perception.

    abstract::Visual motion serves as a cue for high-level percepts. The present study reports novel modulation of animacy perception through synchronous motion. A target dot moving along a random trajectory was presented. The trajectory was generated based on a variant of 1/f noise; hence, the dot could be perceived as animate. Pa...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.8.17

    authors: Takahashi K,Watanabe K

    更新日期:2015-01-01 00:00:00