Motion-induced position shift in stereoscopic and dichoptic viewing.

Abstract:

:The static envelope of a Gabor patch with a moving sinusoidal carrier appears shifted in the direction of the carrier motion (De Valois & De Valois, 1991). This phenomenon is called motion-induced position shift. Although several motion-processing stages, ranging from low- to high-level processes, may contribute to position estimation, it is unknown whether a binocular matching stage or an even earlier stage exerts an influence. To elucidate this matter, we investigated the disparity tuning of this illusion by manipulating the binocular disparities of the carrier and the envelope. If the mechanisms underlying the illusion have disparity selectivity, the illusory shift should disappear when the carrier and envelope have sufficiently different disparities. We conducted an experiment in which a sinusoidal grating inside a Gaussian envelope had a crossed or uncrossed disparity and the background was filled with static random noise; each subject correctly judged whether the grating was in front of or behind the fixation plane. Position shift occurred even when the moving carrier had a vastly different disparity from that of the envelope, suggesting that one of the mechanisms responsible for the phenomenon exists at a monocular visual stage. To confirm this, in the next experiment we examined whether depth perception can be produced by an illusory disparity due to illusory position shifts in opposite directions between eyes. Two Gabor-like patches moving in opposite directions were presented at the same retinal position dichoptically. We found that when each monocular patch had a soft edge in its contrast envelope, the depth perception of such a patch was biased toward the depth consistent with the illusory crossed or uncrossed disparity, whereas depth perception of a stimulus with a hard edge was less biased. We suggest that the underlying mechanisms of motion-induced position shift are present at an early stage of monocular visual processing, and that the altered positions are represented in the left-eye and right-eye monocular pathways in a way that allows them to function as tokens of binocular matching.

journal_name

J Vis

journal_title

Journal of vision

authors

Hisakata R,Hayashi D,Murakami I

doi

10.1167/16.13.3

subject

Has Abstract

pub_date

2016-10-01 00:00:00

pages

3

issue

13

issn

1534-7362

pii

2572580

journal_volume

16

pub_type

杂志文章
  • Oculometric assessment of dynamic visual processing.

    abstract::Eye movements are the most frequent (∼3/s), shortest-latency (∼150-250 ms), and biomechanically simplest (one joint, no inertial complexities) voluntary motor behavior in primates, providing a model system to assess sensorimotor disturbances arising from trauma, fatigue, aging, or disease states. We have developed a 1...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.14.12

    authors: Liston DB,Stone LS

    更新日期:2014-12-19 00:00:00

  • The geometric structure of color.

    abstract::Color is commonly described in terms of the three perceptual attributes-hue, saturation, and brightness-of which only hue has a qualitative nature, saturation and brightness being of a quantitative nature. A possible reason for such a phenomenological structure of the color manifold, and its geometric representation, ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.1.16

    authors: Logvinenko AD

    更新日期:2015-01-14 00:00:00

  • Illumination estimation in three-dimensional scenes with and without specular cues.

    abstract::We report the results of three experiments in which observers judged the albedo of surfaces at different locations in rendered, three-dimensional scenes consisting of two rooms connected by a doorway. All surfaces composing the rooms were achromatic and Lambertian, and a gradient of illumination increased with depth. ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/5.10.8

    authors: Snyder JL,Doerschner K,Maloney LT

    更新日期:2005-12-15 00:00:00

  • Contrast-response functions for multifocal visual evoked potentials: a test of a model relating V1 activity to multifocal visual evoked potentials activity.

    abstract::The multifocal visual evoked potential (mfVEP) is largely generated in V1. To relate the electrical activity recorded from humans to recordings from single cells in nonhuman primate (V1) cortex, contrast-response functions for the human mfVEP were compared to predictions from a model of V1 activity (D. J. Heeger, A. C...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/6.5.4

    authors: Hood DC,Ghadiali Q,Zhang JC,Graham NV,Wolfson SS,Zhang X

    更新日期:2006-04-24 00:00:00

  • Human sensitivity to perturbations constrained by a model of the natural image manifold.

    abstract::Humans are remarkably well tuned to the statistical properties of natural images. However, quantitative characterization of processing within the domain of natural images has been difficult because most parametric manipulations of a natural image make that image appear less natural. We used generative adversarial netw...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/18.11.20

    authors: Fruend I,Stalker E

    更新日期:2018-10-01 00:00:00

  • Induction from a below-threshold chromatic pattern.

    abstract::Patterned backgrounds can induce large shifts in color appearance, even with patterns of only 10% S-cone contrast (S. K. Shevell & P. Monnier, 2005). The present study tested whether a background pattern could induce color shifts even at a below-threshold contrast. In the first experiment, S-cone contrast threshold fo...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.12.7

    authors: Monnier P,Shevell SK

    更新日期:2008-09-23 00:00:00

  • Does visual flicker phase at gamma frequency modulate neural signal propagation and stimulus selection?

    abstract::Oscillatory synchronization of neuronal populations has been proposed to play a role in perceptual integration and attentional processing. However, some conflicting evidence has been found with respect to its causal relevance for sensory processing, particularly when using flickering visual stimuli with the aim of dri...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/12.4.5

    authors: Bauer M,Akam T,Joseph S,Freeman E,Driver J

    更新日期:2012-04-13 00:00:00

  • The detection of multiple global directions: capacity limits with spatially segregated and transparent-motion signals.

    abstract::An important constraint on motion processing is the maximum number of directions that can be perceived at the same time. When transparent-motion stimuli are constructed based solely on direction differences, prior studies demonstrate that no more than two directions are seen simultaneously. However, this limit has bee...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.1.40

    authors: Greenwood JA,Edwards M

    更新日期:2009-01-30 00:00:00

  • Monitoring mouse retinal degeneration with high-resolution spectral-domain optical coherence tomography.

    abstract::Progression of retinal degeneration in a mouse model was studied in vivo with high-resolution spectral-domain optical coherence tomography (SD-OCT). Imaging in 3D with high depth resolution (<3 mum), SD-OCT resolved all the major layers of the retina of control C57BL/6J mice. Images of transgenic mice having a null mu...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.1.17

    authors: Kim KH,Puoris'haag M,Maguluri GN,Umino Y,Cusato K,Barlow RB,de Boer JF

    更新日期:2008-01-24 00:00:00

  • Larger visual stimuli are perceived to last longer from time to time: The internal clock is not affected by nontemporal visual stimulus size.

    abstract::Performance on interval timing is often explained by the assumption of an internal clock based on neural counting. According to this account, a neural pacemaker generates pulses, and the number of pulses relating to a physical time interval is recorded by a counter. Thus, the number of accumulated pulses is the intern...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.3.5

    authors: Rammsayer TH,Verner M

    更新日期:2015-03-10 00:00:00

  • Sustained directional biases in motion transparency.

    abstract::In motion transparency, one surface is very often seen on top of the other in spite of no proper depth cue in the display. We investigated the dynamics of depth assignment in motion transparency stimuli composed of random dots moving in opposite directions. Similarly to other bistable percepts, which surface is seen i...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.13.23

    authors: Mamassian P,Wallace JM

    更新日期:2010-11-01 00:00:00

  • Noise masking of S-cone increments and decrements.

    abstract::S-cone increment and decrement detection thresholds were measured in the presence of bipolar, dynamic noise masks. Noise chromaticities were the L-, M-, and S-cone directions, as well as L-M, L+M, and achromatic (L+M+S) directions. Noise contrast power was varied to measure threshold Energy versus Noise (EvN) function...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.13.8

    authors: Wang Q,Richters DP,Eskew RT Jr

    更新日期:2014-11-12 00:00:00

  • Crowding and perceptual organization: Target's objecthood influences the relative strength of part-level and configural-level crowding.

    abstract::In visual crowding, identification of a peripheral object is impaired by nearby objects. Recent studies have demonstrated that crowding is not limited only to interaction between low-level features or parts, as presumed by most models of crowding, but can also occur between high-level, configural representations of ob...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.11.7

    authors: Pirkner Y,Kimchi R

    更新日期:2017-09-01 00:00:00

  • Cross-orientation masking is speed invariant between ocular pathways but speed dependent within them.

    abstract::In human (D. H. Baker, T. S. Meese, & R. J. Summers, 2007b) and in cat (B. Li, M. R. Peterson, J. K. Thompson, T. Duong, & R. D. Freeman, 2005; F. Sengpiel & V. Vorobyov, 2005) there are at least two routes to cross-orientation suppression (XOS): a broadband, non-adaptable, monocular (within-eye) pathway and a more na...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.5.2

    authors: Meese TS,Baker DH

    更新日期:2009-05-04 00:00:00

  • Partial modal completion under occlusion: what do modal and amodal percepts represent?

    abstract::In the occlusion illusion, a partly occluded object is perceived as though it were less occluded than it actually is (Palmer, Brooks, & Lai, 2007). We confirm and extend this finding using a stimulus with a moving occluder. In agreement with Palmer et al.'s (2007) findings and their partial-modal-completion hypothesis...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.1.22

    authors: Scherzer TR,Ekroll V

    更新日期:2015-01-22 00:00:00

  • Effect of overlaid luminance contrast on perceived color contrast: Shadows enhance, borders suppress.

    abstract::Natural scenes contain both color and luminance variations at different sizes and orientations that are sometimes spatially overlaid and sometimes not. Here, we explore visual interactions between overlaid color and luminance contrast that are both suprathreshold and highly visible. We used a color-luminance plaid in ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/16.11.15

    authors: Kim YJ,Mullen KT

    更新日期:2016-09-01 00:00:00

  • Numerosity adaptation partly depends on the allocation of implicit numerosity-contingent visuo-spatial attention.

    abstract::Like other perceptual attributes, numerosity is susceptible to adaptation. Nevertheless, it has never been fully investigated whether adaptation to numerosity is fully perceptual in nature or if it stems from the mixed influence of perception and attention. In the present work, we addressed this point throughout three...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/jov.21.1.12

    authors: Grasso PA,Anobile G,Arrighi R

    更新日期:2021-01-04 00:00:00

  • Davida Teller Award Lecture 2017: What can be learned from natural behavior?

    abstract::The essentially active nature of vision has long been acknowledged but has been difficult to investigate because of limitations in the available instrumentation, both for measuring eye and body movements and for presenting realistic stimuli in the context of active behavior. These limitations have been substantially r...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 传,历史文章,杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/18.4.10

    authors: Hayhoe MM

    更新日期:2018-04-01 00:00:00

  • Abnormality in face scanning by children with autism spectrum disorder is limited to the eye region: evidence from multi-method analyses of eye tracking data.

    abstract::There has been considerable controversy regarding whether children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing children (TD) show different eye movement patterns when processing faces. We investigated ASD and age- and IQ-matched TD children's scanning of faces using a novel multi-method approach. We f...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/13.10.5

    authors: Yi L,Fan Y,Quinn PC,Feng C,Huang D,Li J,Mao G,Lee K

    更新日期:2013-08-08 00:00:00

  • The motion-induced shift in the perceived location of a grating also shifts its aftereffect.

    abstract::Motion can bias the perceived location of a stationary stimulus (Whitney & Cavanagh, 2000), but whether this occurs at a high level of representation or at early, retinotopic stages of visual processing remains an open question. As coding of orientation emerges early in visual processing, we tested whether motion coul...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/12.8.7

    authors: Kosovicheva AA,Maus GW,Anstis S,Cavanagh P,Tse PU,Whitney D

    更新日期:2012-01-01 00:00:00

  • Accuracy and precision of objective refraction from wavefront aberrations.

    abstract::We determined the accuracy and precision of 33 objective methods for predicting the results of conventional, sphero-cylindrical refraction from wavefront aberrations in a large population of 200 eyes. Accuracy for predicting defocus (as specified by the population mean error of prediction) varied from -0.50 D to +0.25...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/4.4.9

    authors: Thibos LN,Hong X,Bradley A,Applegate RA

    更新日期:2004-04-23 00:00:00

  • Perception-memory interactions reveal a computational strategy for perceptual constancy.

    abstract::A key challenge for the visual system is to extract constant object properties from incoming sensory information. This information is ambiguous because the same sensory signal can arise from many combinations of object properties and viewing conditions and noisy because of the variability in sensory encoding. The comp...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/16.3.38

    authors: Olkkonen M,Saarela TP,Allred SR

    更新日期:2016-01-01 00:00:00

  • Motion perception by a moving observer in a three-dimensional environment.

    abstract::Perceiving three-dimensional object motion while moving through the world is hard: not only must optic flow be segmented and parallax resolved into shape and motion, but also observer motion needs to be taken into account in order to perceive absolute, rather than observer-relative motion. In order to simplify the las...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/13.2.15

    authors: Dupin L,Wexler M

    更新日期:2013-02-08 00:00:00

  • Where do we store the memory representations that guide attention?

    abstract::During the last decade one of the most contentious and heavily studied topics in the attention literature has been the role that working memory representations play in controlling perceptual selection. The hypothesis has been advanced that to have attention select a certain perceptual input from the environment, we on...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章,评审

    doi:10.1167/13.3.1

    authors: Woodman GF,Carlisle NB,Reinhart RM

    更新日期:2013-02-25 00:00:00

  • Inferring the stiffness of unfamiliar objects from optical, shape, and motion cues.

    abstract::Visually inferring the stiffness of objects is important for many tasks but is challenging because, unlike optical properties (e.g., gloss), mechanical properties do not directly affect image values. Stiffness must be inferred either (a) by recognizing materials and recalling their properties (associative approach) or...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.3.18

    authors: Schmidt F,Paulun VC,van Assen JJ,Fleming RW

    更新日期:2017-03-01 00:00:00

  • Methods for determining equiluminance in terms of L/M cone ratios.

    abstract::Heterochromatic flicker photometry (HFP), minimum motion (MM), and minimally distinct border (MDB) settings have often been used to determine equiluminance, a relative intensity setting for two chromaticities that, in theory, eliminates the responses of a luminance or achromatic psychophysical mechanism. These setting...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/jov.20.4.22

    authors: He J,Taveras Cruz Y,Eskew RT Jr

    更新日期:2020-04-09 00:00:00

  • Spatial relationships between contours impact rapid scene classification.

    abstract::Photographs and line drawings of natural scenes are easily classified even when the image is only briefly visible to the observer. Contour junctions and points of high curvature have been shown to be important for perceptual organization (Attneave, 1954; Biederman, 1987) and have been proposed to be influential in rap...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/18.8.1

    authors: Wilder J,Dickinson S,Jepson A,Walther DB

    更新日期:2018-08-01 00:00:00

  • Origin of the fast negative ERG component from isolated aspartate-treated mouse retina.

    abstract::The leading edge of the a-wave of the ERG is generally believed to accurately reflect the changes in the circulating current through the cGMP-gated channels in the outer segment plasma membrane of rods and cones. The aspartate-isolated mammalian electroretinogram (ERG) to a rod-saturating flash contains a fast "nose"-...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.12.9

    authors: Vinberg FJ,Strandman S,Koskelainen A

    更新日期:2009-11-17 00:00:00

  • Simulating prosthetic vision: Optimizing the information content of a limited visual display.

    abstract::Visual prostheses for the restoration of functional vision are currently under development. To guide prosthesis research and allow for an accurate prognosis of functional gain, simulating the experience of a retinal prosthesis in healthy individuals is desirable. Current simulation paradigms lack crucial aspects of th...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.14.32

    authors: van Rheede JJ,Kennard C,Hicks SL

    更新日期:2010-12-29 00:00:00

  • Monocular occlusions determine the perceived shape and depth of occluding surfaces.

    abstract::Recent experiments have established that monocular areas arising due to occlusion of one object by another contribute to stereoscopic depth perception. It has been suggested that the primary role of monocular occlusions is to define depth discontinuities and object boundaries in depth. Here we use a carefully designed...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.6.11

    authors: Tsirlin I,Wilcox LM,Allison RS

    更新日期:2010-06-01 00:00:00