The distance used for scaling disparities is the same as the one used for scaling retinal size.

Abstract:

:To determine the physical size and global three-dimensional (3-D) shape of an object, retinal size and retinal disparity have to be scaled in accordance with the object's distance. We examined whether the distance used for scaling retinal disparity is the same as the distance used for scaling retinal size. Subjects adjusted the 3-D shape (size and depth) of a computer-simulated ellipsoid to match a tennis ball. Analysis of the errors when only the ellipsoid was visible in an otherwise completely dark room suggests that the distance used for scaling retinal disparity is indeed the same as that used for scaling retinal size. This was confirmed by showing that the correspondence between the distance used for scaling retinal disparity and that used for scaling retinal size does not improve when more information about distance is available (room lights on), although both distances are then much closer to the simulated distance. Finally, we show that this correspondence is not due to the use of distance-invariant higher order binocular information.

journal_name

Vision Res

journal_title

Vision research

authors

van Damme W,Brenner E

doi

10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00213-1

subject

Has Abstract

pub_date

1997-03-01 00:00:00

pages

757-64

issue

6

eissn

0042-6989

issn

1878-5646

pii

S0042-6989(96)00213-1

journal_volume

37

pub_type

杂志文章
  • Oculomotor and skeletal motor systems share one map of visual space.

    abstract::Previous research has shown that cognitive and motor visual systems have separate maps of visual space because spatial values in them can be manipulated independently. Here we ask whether the motor map in turn can be divided into separate representations of space. Forty large saccadic eye movements in darkness resulte...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/0042-6989(87)90088-5

    authors: Nemire K,Bridgeman B

    更新日期:1987-01-01 00:00:00

  • Active control of the visual field in the starfish Acanthaster planci.

    abstract::Photoreception in echinoderms has been studied for several years with a focus on the dermal photoreceptors of echinoids. Even though spatial vision has been proposed for this dermal photosystem, by far the most advanced system is found in a number of asteroids where an unpaired tube foot at the tip of each arm carries...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2016.05.007

    authors: Beer S,Wentzel C,Petie R,Garm A

    更新日期:2016-10-01 00:00:00

  • Dependence of the chromatic valence function on chromatic standards.

    abstract::The red, green, yellow, and blue curve portions of chromatic valence functions (CVFs) were measured by an iso-cancellation technique using three or four different chromatic standards (cancellation stimuli of fixed intensity and chromaticity) for each of the four curve portions. Curve shapes of the red, green, and yell...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/0042-6989(89)90070-9

    authors: Ayama M,Ikeda M

    更新日期:1989-01-01 00:00:00

  • Monocular deprivation in kittens differently affects crossed and uncrossed visual pathways.

    abstract::The effects of monocular deprivation (MD) on the crossed and uncrossed visual projections were studied using both electrophysiological and behavioural criteria. Our results show that Visual Evoked Potentials (VEPs) from the deprived eye (DE) in response to contrast reversing gratings are more reduced in the ipsilatera...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/0042-6989(86)90145-8

    authors: Bisti S,Carmignoto G

    更新日期:1986-01-01 00:00:00

  • Saccadic countermanding: a comparison of central and peripheral stop signals.

    abstract::We compared the effectiveness of central and peripheral targets in a saccadic countermanding task. Stop-signal reaction times (SSRTs) do not differ significantly for central and peripheral stop signals. Further, when central and peripheral stop signals are presented together, SSRTs behave as expected of independent pr...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00107-9

    authors: Asrress KN,Carpenter RH

    更新日期:2001-09-01 00:00:00

  • Attentive pointing in natural scenes correlates with other measures of attention.

    abstract::Finger pointing is a natural human behavior frequently used to draw attention to specific parts of sensory input. Since this pointing behavior is likely preceded and/or accompanied by the deployment of attention by the pointing person, we hypothesize that pointing can be used as a natural means of providing self-repor...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2017.04.001

    authors: Jeck DM,Qin M,Egeth H,Niebur E

    更新日期:2017-06-01 00:00:00

  • Cortical dynamics of form and motion integration: persistence, apparent motion, and illusory contours.

    abstract::How does the visual system generate percepts of moving forms? How does this happen when the forms are emergent percepts, such as illusory contours or segregated textures, and the motion percept is apparent motion between the emergent forms? We develop a neural model of form-motion interactions to explain and simulate ...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/0042-6989(95)00052-2

    authors: Francis G,Grossberg S

    更新日期:1996-01-01 00:00:00

  • The flash-lag effect is reduced when the flash is perceived as a sensory consequence of our action.

    abstract::The flash-lag effect (FLE) is defined as an error in localization that consists of perceiving a flashed object to lag behind a moving one when both are presented in physical alignment. Previous studies have addressed the question if it is the predictability of the flash, or the moving object, that modulates the amount...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.11.016

    authors: López-Moliner J,Linares D

    更新日期:2006-06-01 00:00:00

  • In vivo biometry in the mouse eye with low coherence interferometry.

    abstract:PURPOSE:A major drawback of the mouse model of myopia is that the ocular dimensions cannot be measured in vivo, and that histological techniques post-mortem suffer from limited resolution. We have tested the potential of a newly developed technique, optical low coherence interferometry (OLCI), adapted for short measure...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.05.018

    authors: Schmucker C,Schaeffel F

    更新日期:2004-01-01 00:00:00

  • Contextual effects in speed perception may occur at an early stage of processing.

    abstract::How does nearby motion affect the perceived speed of a target region? When a central drifting Gabor patch is surrounded by translating noise, its speed can be misperceived over a fourfold range. Typically, when a surround moves in the same direction, perceived centre speed is reduced; for opposite-direction surrounds ...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.11.011

    authors: Baker DH,Graf EW

    更新日期:2010-01-01 00:00:00

  • A system of regular ridges instead of nipples on a compound eye that has to operate near the diffraction limit.

    abstract::A system of regular, radial ridges, spaced approx. 250 nm apart, is reported from the outer corneal surface of the eye of the tiny moth Leucoptera coffeella. Antireflective coatings in larger insects with flatter facets often take the form of corneal nipples. However, evidence is presented that in an insect whose eyes...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/0042-6989(93)90223-j

    authors: Meyer-Rochow VB,Stringer IA

    更新日期:1993-12-01 00:00:00

  • Greater losses in sensitivity to second-order local motion than to first-order local motion after early visual deprivation in humans.

    abstract::We compared sensitivity to first-order versus second-order local motion in patients treated for dense central congenital cataracts in one or both eyes. Amplitude modulation thresholds were measured for discriminating the direction of motion of luminance-modulated (first-order) and contrast modulated (second-order) hor...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.11.019

    authors: Ellemberg D,Lewis TL,Defina N,Maurer D,Brent HP,Guillemot JP,Lepore F

    更新日期:2005-10-01 00:00:00

  • Coding of the contrasts in natural images by populations of neurons in primary visual cortex (V1).

    abstract::It is possible to discriminate between grating contrasts over a 300-fold contrast range, whereas V1 neurons have very limited dynamic ranges. Using populations of model neurons with contrast-response parameters taken from electrophysiological studies (cat and macaque), we investigated ways of combining responses to co...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(03)00277-3

    authors: Clatworthy PL,Chirimuuta M,Lauritzen JS,Tolhurst DJ

    更新日期:2003-08-01 00:00:00

  • Possible role of the Müller cell in uptake and metabolism of glutamate in the mammalian outer retina.

    abstract::It is not known how glutamate released synaptically in the outer plexiform layer of the retina is inactivated. Since there is no extracellular enzymatic degradation, glutamate released from photoreceptors is taken up intracellularly by at least one of the three transporters known, and metabolized by a glutamate degrad...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00140-x

    authors: Derouiche A

    更新日期:1996-12-01 00:00:00

  • Uneven mapping of magnocellular and parvocellular projections from the lateral geniculate nucleus to the striate cortex in the macaque monkey.

    abstract::Central vision is substantially over represented in the lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN) and striate cortex. The over representation could be accompanied by a selective expansion of central vision in parvocellular dLGN, in which case the ratio of parvocellular to magnocellular inputs to striate cortex should change w...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00319-8

    authors: Azzopardi P,Jones KE,Cowey A

    更新日期:1999-06-01 00:00:00

  • Stimulus specificity in spatially-extended interocular suppression.

    abstract::In typical binocular rivalry demonstrations, disparate images presented in corresponding locations to the two eyes are found to alternate perceptually over time. Alternation in perception can occur even if the images presented to the two eyes do not overlap, if they are sufficiently close in space. This implies a spat...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章,随机对照试验

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.001

    authors: Nichols DF,Wilson HR

    更新日期:2009-07-01 00:00:00

  • Change in feature space is not necessary for the flash-lag effect.

    abstract::To achieve perceptual match between a flashed target and a gradually changing one, the flashed target should have the feature value corresponding to the value to be obtained by the gradually changing target only later. Flashed target should be positioned ahead of the continuously moving one in order to be perceived as...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00003-7

    authors: Bachmann T,Põder E

    更新日期:2001-04-01 00:00:00

  • Centering biases in heterochromatic brightness matching.

    abstract::When the method of constant stimuli is used to measure heterochromatic brightness matches, the resulting matches can be strongly biased toward the center of the range of test luminances used. In the present paper, we investigate the source of this centering bias. The stimuli were 2 degrees red squares presented in a g...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.06.019

    authors: Pereverzeva M,Teller DY

    更新日期:2005-11-01 00:00:00

  • Morphology and spatial arrangement of large retinal ganglion cells projecting to the optic tectum in the perciform fish Pholidapus dybowskii.

    abstract::Using retrograde HRP labeling from the optic nerve (ON) or optic tectum (OT), we have visualized large ganglion cells (LGCs) in wholemounted retinas of the teleost Pholidapus dybowskii and studied their morphology and spatial properties. In all, three LGC types were distinguished. In a previous paper, detailed data we...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2007.07.004

    authors: Pushchin II,Podugolnikova TA,Kondrashev SL

    更新日期:2007-11-01 00:00:00

  • Altered white matter in early visual pathways of humans with amblyopia.

    abstract::Amblyopia is a visual disorder caused by poorly coordinated binocular input during development. Little is known about the impact of amblyopia on the white matter within the visual system. We studied the properties of six major visual white-matter pathways in a group of adults with amblyopia (n=10) and matched controls...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.12.021

    authors: Allen B,Spiegel DP,Thompson B,Pestilli F,Rokers B

    更新日期:2015-09-01 00:00:00

  • Spatial summation in dark-adapted human infants.

    abstract::Behavioral measures of spatial summation in 4- and 11-week old human infants were obtained using the forced-choice preferential looking technique. Infants exhibit complete spatial summation over large areas (4 weeks: 8.9 degrees; 11 weeks: 5.5 degrees), much larger than those of adults tested in the same apparatus (2....

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/0042-6989(84)90146-9

    authors: Hamer RD,Schneck ME

    更新日期:1984-01-01 00:00:00

  • Two models of experimental myopia in the mouse.

    abstract:PURPOSE:The purpose of this study was to test the response of the mouse eye to two methods for the induction of experimental myopia. METHODS:Growth patterns of eyes were determined by axial length measurements from birth to adult in eyes of both sexes of normal mice examined on post-natal day 1 to 6 months and at 1 ye...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.01.004

    authors: Barathi VA,Boopathi VG,Yap EP,Beuerman RW

    更新日期:2008-03-01 00:00:00

  • Position displacement, not velocity, is the cue to motion detection of second-order stimuli.

    abstract::Motion detection can be achieved either with mechanisms sensitive to a target's velocity, or sensitive to change in a target's position. Using a procedure to dissociate these two provided by Nakayama and Tyler (Vis Res 1981;21:427-433), we explored detection of first-order (luminance-based) and various second-order (t...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00035-2

    authors: Seiffert AE,Cavanagh P

    更新日期:1998-11-01 00:00:00

  • Combining information in different color-coding mechanisms to facilitate visual search.

    abstract::An earlier experiment using a yes-no procedure with a search accuracy task [A.L. Nagy, G. Thomas, Distractor heterogeneity, attention, and color in visual search tasks, Vision Research, 43 (2003) 1541-1552] showed that observers could combine information in different cardinal color mechanisms to facilitate search perf...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.05.031

    authors: Nagy AL,Young T,Neriani K

    更新日期:2004-11-01 00:00:00

  • The spatial window of the perceptual template and endogenous attention.

    abstract::One primary function of spatial attention is to exclude external noise [e.g., Psychol. Sci. 11(2) (2000) 139], especially in the region of the target stimulus [J. Vis. 2(4) (2000) 312]. What is not known is the spatial profile of external noise exclusion in the vicinity of the target and how this depends upon attentio...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.01.011

    authors: Dosher BA,Liu SH,Blair N,Lu ZL

    更新日期:2004-06-01 00:00:00

  • Revisiting motion repulsion: evidence for a general phenomenon?

    abstract::Previous studies have found large misperceptions when subjects are reporting the perceived angle between two directions of motion moving transparently at an acute angle, the so called motion repulsion. While these errors have been assumed to be caused by interactions between the two directions present, we reassessed t...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00025-5

    authors: Rauber HJ,Treue S

    更新日期:1999-09-01 00:00:00

  • Effects of longitudinal chromatic aberration on accommodation and emmetropization.

    abstract:PURPOSE:Less accommodation was found when human subjects read in blue (peak at about 440 nm) than when they read in red light (above 600 nm; [Kroger & Binder, British Journal of Ophthalmology 84 (2000) 890]). On the other hand, emmetropization in chickens did not appear to compensate for the chromatic defocus (385 nm v...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00262-6

    authors: Seidemann A,Schaeffel F

    更新日期:2002-09-01 00:00:00

  • Similarity-based distortion of visual short-term memory is due to perceptual averaging.

    abstract::A task-irrelevant stimulus can distort recall from visual short-term memory (VSTM). Specifically, reproduction of a task-relevant memory item is biased in the direction of the irrelevant memory item (Huang & Sekuler, 2010a). The present study addresses the hypothesis that such effects reflect the influence of neural a...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/j.visres.2013.12.016

    authors: Dubé C,Zhou F,Kahana MJ,Sekuler R

    更新日期:2014-03-01 00:00:00

  • Colored neon flanks and line gap enhancement.

    abstract::When a colored line connects two black (or differently colored) lines across a gap, colored neon flanks are seen on either side of it. These flanks extend over gap sizes of 50 min arc foveally and are not explained by Bezold-type assimilation. They may be elicited by black lines as short as 6 min arc adjoining the col...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/0042-6989(84)90185-8

    authors: Redies C,Spillmann L,Kunz K

    更新日期:1984-01-01 00:00:00

  • Habituation-like effects cause a significant decrease in response in MRI neuroactivation during visual stimulation.

    abstract::A wide range of rest/stimulus cycle durations (40-360 sec) is reported to have been used by various groups for MRI neuroactivation studies of the visual cortex. In this paper we demonstrate a clear habituation-like response for longer cycle durations which results in a halving of apparent activation between cycle dura...

    journal_title:Vision research

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(97)85231-5

    authors: Condon B,McFadzean R,Hadley DM,Bradnam MS,Shahani U

    更新日期:1997-05-01 00:00:00