Mach bands and multiscale models of spatial vision: the role of first, second, and third derivative operators in encoding bars and edges.

Abstract:

:Ernst Mach observed that light or dark bands could be seen at abrupt changes of luminance gradient in the absence of peaks or troughs in luminance. Many models of feature detection share the idea that bars, lines, and Mach bands are found at peaks and troughs in the output of even-symmetric spatial filters. Our experiments assessed the appearance of Mach bands (position and width) and the probability of seeing them on a novel set of generalized Gaussian edges. Mach band probability was mainly determined by the shape of the luminance profile and increased with the sharpness of its corners, controlled by a single parameter (n). Doubling or halving the size of the images had no significant effect. Variations in contrast (20%-80%) and duration (50-300 ms) had relatively minor effects. These results rule out the idea that Mach bands depend simply on the amplitude of the second derivative, but a multiscale model, based on Gaussian-smoothed first- and second-derivative filtering, can account accurately for the probability and perceived spatial layout of the bands. A key idea is that Mach band visibility depends on the ratio of second- to first-derivative responses at peaks in the second-derivative scale-space map. This ratio is approximately scale-invariant and increases with the sharpness of the corners of the luminance ramp, as observed. The edges of Mach bands pose a surprisingly difficult challenge for models of edge detection, but a nonlinear third-derivative operation is shown to predict the locations of Mach band edges strikingly well. Mach bands thus shed new light on the role of multiscale filtering systems in feature coding.

journal_name

J Vis

journal_title

Journal of vision

authors

Wallis SA,Georgeson MA

doi

10.1167/12.13.18

subject

Has Abstract

pub_date

2012-12-21 00:00:00

pages

18

issue

13

issn

1534-7362

pii

12.13.18

journal_volume

12

pub_type

杂志文章
  • Slow gain fluctuations limit benefits of temporal integration in visual cortex.

    abstract::Sensory neurons represent stimulus information with sequences of action potentials that differ across repeated measurements. This variability limits the information that can be extracted from momentary observations of a neuron's response. It is often assumed that integrating responses over time mitigates this limitati...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/18.8.8

    authors: Goris RLT,Ziemba CM,Movshon JA,Simoncelli EP

    更新日期:2018-08-01 00:00:00

  • A single "stopwatch" for duration estimation, a single "ruler" for size.

    abstract::Although observers can discriminate visual targets with long exposures from otherwise-identical targets with shorter exposures, temporally overlapping distracters with an intermediate exposure can produce a striking degradation in performance. This new finding suggests that observers can only estimate one duration at ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.2.14

    authors: Morgan MJ,Giora E,Solomon JA

    更新日期:2008-02-29 00:00:00

  • Collinear integration affects visual search at V1.

    abstract::Perceptual grouping plays an indispensable role in figure-ground segregation and attention distribution. For example, a column pops out if it contains element bars orthogonal to uniformly oriented element bars. Jingling and Tseng (2013) have reported that contextual grouping in a column matters to visual search behavi...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/13.10.24

    authors: Chow HM,Jingling L,Tseng CH

    更新日期:2013-08-29 00:00:00

  • A role for incidental auditory learning in auditory-visual word learning among kindergarten children.

    abstract::This study focused on the potential role of incidental, auditory perceptual learning in among children learning new words. To this end, we examined how irrelevant auditory similarities across words, that provide no cues regarding their visual or conceptual attributes, influence pseudo-word learning in a name/picture m...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/jovi.20.3.4

    authors: Banai K,Nir B,Moav-Scheff R,Bar-Ziv N

    更新日期:2020-03-17 00:00:00

  • Transfer in motion discrimination learning was no greater in double training than in single training.

    abstract::We investigated the controversy regarding double training in motion discrimination learning. We collected data from 43 participants in a motion direction discrimination learning task with either double training (i.e., training plus exposure) or single training (i.e., no exposure). By pooling these data with those in t...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.6.7

    authors: Huang J,Liang J,Zhou Y,Liu Z

    更新日期:2017-06-01 00:00:00

  • Combining local and global limitations of visual search.

    abstract::There are different opinions about the roles of local interactions and central processing capacity in visual search. This study attempts to clarify the problem using a new version of relevant set cueing. A central precue indicates two symmetrical segments (that may contain a target object) within a circular array of o...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.4.10

    authors: Põder E

    更新日期:2017-04-01 00:00:00

  • Differential effects of visual attention and working memory on binocular rivalry.

    abstract::The investigation of cognitive influence on binocular rivalry has a long history. However, the effects of visual WM on rivalry have never been studied so far. We examined top-down modulation of rivalry perception in four experiments to compare the effects of visual WM and sustained selective attention: In the first th...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.5.13

    authors: Scocchia L,Valsecchi M,Gegenfurtner KR,Triesch J

    更新日期:2014-05-30 00:00:00

  • Low spatial frequencies are suppressively masked across spatial scale, orientation, field position, and eye of origin.

    abstract::Masking is said to occur when a mask stimulus interferes with the visibility of a target (test) stimulus. One widely held view of this process supposes interactions between mask and test mechanisms (cross-channel masking), and explicit models (e.g., J. M. Foley, 1994) have proposed that the interactions are inhibitory...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/4.10.2

    authors: Meese TS,Hess RF

    更新日期:2004-10-18 00:00:00

  • A method to characterize compensatory oculomotor strategies following simulated central vision loss.

    abstract::Loss of central vision can be partially compensated by increased use of peripheral vision. For example, patients experiencing central vision loss due to disease (macular degeneration) or healthy participants trained with simulated central vision loss, tend to develop eccentric fixation spots for reading or other visua...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/jov.20.9.15

    authors: Maniglia M,Visscher KM,Seitz AR

    更新日期:2020-09-02 00:00:00

  • Living up to optimal expectations.

    abstract::Natural visual scenes contain several independent sources of information (cues) about a single property such as slant. It is widely assumed that the visual system processes such cues separately and then combines them with an averaging operation that takes the reliabilities of the individual cues into account. Does tha...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/7.3.2

    authors: Muller CM,Brenner E,Smeets JB

    更新日期:2007-02-16 00:00:00

  • Retinotopic encoding of the Ternus-Pikler display reflected in the early visual areas.

    abstract::The visual representation of the world is often assumed to be retinotopic, and many visual brain areas are indeed organized retinotopically. Visual perception, however, is not based on a reference frame anchored in retinotopic coordinates. For example, when an object moves, motion of its constituent parts is perceived...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/16.3.26

    authors: Thunell E,van der Zwaag W,Ögmen H,Plomp G,Herzog MH

    更新日期:2016-01-01 00:00:00

  • Global motion processing: The effect of spatial scale and eccentricity.

    abstract::Here we examine how global translational motion sensitivity varies with the spatial frequency of the elements in local motion and on the eccentricity of stimulation. Using DC-balanced, spatially narrowband elements (radial log Gabors) matched in terms of multiples above contrast threshold, we show that global translat...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.4.11

    authors: Hess RF,Aaen-Stockdale C

    更新日期:2008-04-17 00:00:00

  • Oculometric assessment of dynamic visual processing.

    abstract::Eye movements are the most frequent (∼3/s), shortest-latency (∼150-250 ms), and biomechanically simplest (one joint, no inertial complexities) voluntary motor behavior in primates, providing a model system to assess sensorimotor disturbances arising from trauma, fatigue, aging, or disease states. We have developed a 1...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.14.12

    authors: Liston DB,Stone LS

    更新日期:2014-12-19 00:00:00

  • Manual tracking enhances smooth pursuit eye movements.

    abstract::Previous studies have reported that concurrent manual tracking enhances smooth pursuit eye movements only when tracking a self-driven or a predictable moving target. Here, we used a control-theoretic approach to examine whether concurrent manual tracking enhances smooth pursuit of an unpredictable moving target. In th...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.15.11

    authors: Niehorster DC,Siu WW,Li L

    更新日期:2015-01-01 00:00:00

  • Beyond distance and direction: the brain represents target locations non-metrically.

    abstract::In their day-to-day activities human beings are constantly generating behavior, such as pointing, grasping or verbal reports, on the basis of visible target locations. The question arises how the brain represents target locations. One possibility is that the brain represents them metrically, i.e. in terms of distance ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.3.3

    authors: Thaler L,Goodale MA

    更新日期:2010-03-23 00:00:00

  • Prior depth information can bias motion perception.

    abstract::Previous studies of the motion aperture problem have shown that the direction of grating motion can be biased by using binocular disparity to designate borders of the aperture as intrinsic (belonging to the grating) or extrinsic (resulting from occlusion of the grating). Observers report motion in the direction of the...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/4.6.2

    authors: Graf EW,Adams WJ,Lages M

    更新日期:2004-05-19 00:00:00

  • Adaptation improves performance on a visual search task.

    abstract::Temporal context, or adaptation, profoundly affects visual perception. Despite the strength and prevalence of adaptation effects, their functional role in visual processing remains unclear. The effects of spatial context and their functional role are better understood: these effects highlight features that differ from...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/13.2.6

    authors: Wissig SC,Patterson CA,Kohn A

    更新日期:2013-02-06 00:00:00

  • Effect of light level on the reference frames of visual motion processing.

    abstract::It is empirically known that some action-related visual tasks, which may rely on the construction of spatiotopic coordinates, are not well conducted under mesopic vision. The aim of this study was to clarify the effect of light level on the reference frame, such as retinotopic and spatiotopic coordinate bases, associa...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.13.6

    authors: Yoshimoto S,Uchida-Ota M,Takeuchi T

    更新日期:2014-11-06 00:00:00

  • High reward enhances perceptual learning.

    abstract::Studies of perceptual learning have revealed a great deal of plasticity in adult humans. In this study, we systematically investigated the effects and mechanisms of several forms (trial-by-trial, block, and session rewards) and levels (no, low, high, subliminal) of monetary reward on the rate, magnitude, and generaliz...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/18.8.11

    authors: Zhang P,Hou F,Yan FF,Xi J,Lin BR,Zhao J,Yang J,Chen G,Zhang MY,He Q,Dosher BA,Lu ZL,Huang CB

    更新日期:2018-08-01 00:00:00

  • Sequential perceptual learning of letter identification and "uncrowding" in normal peripheral vision: Effects of task, training order, and cholinergic enhancement.

    abstract::Human adults with normal vision are capable of improving performance on visual tasks through repeated practice. Previous work has shown that enhancing synaptic levels of acetylcholine (ACh) in healthy human adults with donepezil (trade name: Aricept) can increase the magnitude and specificity of perceptual learning (P...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/jov.20.4.24

    authors: Levi DM,Li RW,Silver MA,Chung STL

    更新日期:2020-04-09 00:00:00

  • The cost of making an eye movement: A direct link between visual working memory and saccade execution.

    abstract::To facilitate visual continuity across eye movements, the visual system must presaccadically acquire information about the future foveal image. Previous studies have indicated that visual working memory (VWM) affects saccade execution. However, the reverse relation, the effect of saccade execution on VWM load is less ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.6.15

    authors: Schut MJ,Van der Stoep N,Postma A,Van der Stigchel S

    更新日期:2017-06-01 00:00:00

  • Noise masking of S-cone increments and decrements.

    abstract::S-cone increment and decrement detection thresholds were measured in the presence of bipolar, dynamic noise masks. Noise chromaticities were the L-, M-, and S-cone directions, as well as L-M, L+M, and achromatic (L+M+S) directions. Noise contrast power was varied to measure threshold Energy versus Noise (EvN) function...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.13.8

    authors: Wang Q,Richters DP,Eskew RT Jr

    更新日期:2014-11-12 00:00:00

  • Automatic grouping of regular structures.

    abstract::To cope with the continuously incoming stream of input, the visual system has to group information across space and time. Usually, spatial and temporal grouping are investigated separately. However, recent findings revealed that these two grouping mechanisms strongly interact and should therefore be studied together r...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.8.5

    authors: Hermens F,Scharnowski F,Herzog MH

    更新日期:2010-07-01 00:00:00

  • Induction from a below-threshold chromatic pattern.

    abstract::Patterned backgrounds can induce large shifts in color appearance, even with patterns of only 10% S-cone contrast (S. K. Shevell & P. Monnier, 2005). The present study tested whether a background pattern could induce color shifts even at a below-threshold contrast. In the first experiment, S-cone contrast threshold fo...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.12.7

    authors: Monnier P,Shevell SK

    更新日期:2008-09-23 00:00:00

  • Inhibition of target detection in apparent motion trajectory.

    abstract::Letter discrimination performance is degraded when a letter is presented within an apparent motion (AM) trajectory of a spot. This finding suggests that the internal representation of AM stimuli can perceptually interact with other stimuli. In this study, we demonstrated that AM interference could also occur for patte...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/11.10.2

    authors: Hidaka S,Nagai M,Sekuler AB,Bennett PJ,Gyoba J

    更新日期:2011-09-07 00:00:00

  • Integrated global motion influences smooth pursuit in infants.

    abstract::Smooth pursuit eye movements (SP) were studied in 5- and 9-month-old infants and adults in response to a rhombus oscillating horizontally behind three spatially separated vertical occluders. During motion, the rhombus vertices were never visible. Thus the perception of the global motion of the rhombus required integra...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.11.16

    authors: Kochukhova O,Rosander K

    更新日期:2008-08-22 00:00:00

  • Visual search: the role of peripheral information measured using gaze-contingent displays.

    abstract::Two of the factors limiting progress in understanding the mechanisms of visual search are the difficulty of controlling and manipulating the retinal stimulus when the eyes are free to move and the lack of an ideal observer theory for fixation selection during search. Recently, we developed a method to precisely contro...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/6.9.1

    authors: Geisler WS,Perry JS,Najemnik J

    更新日期:2006-08-03 00:00:00

  • Spatiotemporal feature attribution for the perception of visual size.

    abstract::This study examined the role of spatiotemporal feature attribution in the perception of the visual size of objects. A small or a large leading disk, a test disk of variable size, and a probe disk of a fixed size were sequentially presented at the same position for durations of 16.7 ms with interstimulus intervals of 1...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.8.7

    authors: Kawabe T

    更新日期:2008-06-12 00:00:00

  • The dynamics of sensory buffers: geometric, spatial, and experience-dependent shaping of iconic memory.

    abstract::When a stimulus is presented, its sensory trace decays rapidly, lasting for approximately 1000 ms. This brief and labile memory, referred as iconic memory, serves as a buffer before information is transferred to working memory and executive control. Here we explored the effect of different factors--geometric, spatial,...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.5.9

    authors: Graziano M,Sigman M

    更新日期:2008-05-23 00:00:00

  • Adaptive changes in visual cortex following prolonged contrast reduction.

    abstract::How does prolonged reduction in retinal-image contrast affect visual-contrast coding? Recent evidence indicates that some forms of long-term visual deprivation result in compensatory perceptual and neural changes in the adult visual pathway. It has not been established whether changes due to contrast adaptation are be...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.2.20

    authors: Kwon M,Legge GE,Fang F,Cheong AM,He S

    更新日期:2009-02-25 00:00:00