Sedentary antlion larvae (Neuroptera: Myrmeleontidae) use vibrational cues to modify their foraging strategies.

Abstract:

:Learning abilities are exhibited by many animals, including insects. However, sedentary species are typically believed to have low capacities and requirements for learning. Despite this view, recent studies show that even such inconspicuous organisms as larval antlions, which employ an ambush predation strategy, are capable of learning, although their learning abilities are rather simple, i.e., limited to the association of vibrational cues with the arrival of prey. This study demonstrates, for the first time, that antlion larvae can use vibrational cues for complex modifications of their foraging strategies. Specifically, antlion larvae rapidly learn to differentiate between the vibrational cues associated with prey of different sizes, and they save resources by ignoring smaller prey in favour of larger, more energetically profitable prey. Moreover, antlion larvae can learn to associate vibrational cues with the loss of their prey, and they respond by burying their victims under the sand more often and more rapidly than do individuals with no opportunities to form such associations. These findings provide not only new insights into the cognitive abilities of animals but also support for the optimal foraging strategy concept, suggesting the importance of maximizing fitness output by balancing the costs and benefits of alternative foraging strategies.

journal_name

Anim Cogn

journal_title

Animal cognition

authors

Kuszewska K,Miler K,Filipiak M,Woyciechowski M

doi

10.1007/s10071-016-1000-7

subject

Has Abstract

pub_date

2016-09-01 00:00:00

pages

1037-41

issue

5

eissn

1435-9448

issn

1435-9456

pii

10.1007/s10071-016-1000-7

journal_volume

19

pub_type

杂志文章
  • Social transmission of Pavlovian fear: fear-conditioning by-proxy in related female rats.

    abstract::Pairing a previously neutral conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., a tone) to an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US; e.g., a foot-shock) leads to associative learning such that the tone alone will elicit a conditioned response (e.g., freezing). Individuals can also acquire fear from a social context, such as through observ...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0711-2

    authors: Jones CE,Riha PD,Gore AC,Monfils MH

    更新日期:2014-05-01 00:00:00

  • Who are the real bird brains? Qualitative differences in behavioral flexibility between dogs (Canis familiaris) and pigeons (Columba livia).

    abstract::Pigeons given a simultaneous spatial discrimination reversal, in which a single reversal occurs at the midpoint of each session, consistently show anticipation prior to the reversal as well as perseveration after the reversal, suggesting that they use a less effective cue (time or trial number into the session) than w...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0923-8

    authors: Laude JR,Pattison KF,Rayburn-Reeves RM,Michler DM,Zentall TR

    更新日期:2016-01-01 00:00:00

  • Quantification acuity in spontaneous shoaling decisions of three-spined sticklebacks.

    abstract::The ability to discriminate between different quantities is widespread throughout the animal kingdom, and the underlying mechanisms of quantity discrimination are currently intensely discussed. In contrast, questions elucidating the limits of quantity estimation received rather little attention so far. Here, we examin...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0884-y

    authors: Mehlis M,Thünken T,Bakker TC,Frommen JG

    更新日期:2015-09-01 00:00:00

  • How Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) weigh geometric cues depends on their previous experience.

    abstract::Following passive disorientation, Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) learned to search for a hidden food reward located in one corner of a rectangular-shaped enclosure that contained either identical or distinct features in each corner. Identical features allowed for explicit learning of geometric cues, wherea...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0866-0

    authors: Reichert JF,Kelly DM

    更新日期:2015-07-01 00:00:00

  • Pigeons play the percentages: computation of probability in a bird.

    abstract::The ability to compute probability, previously shown in nonverbal infants, apes, and monkeys, was examined in three experiments with pigeons. After responding to individually presented keys in an operant chamber that delivered reinforcement with varying probabilities, pigeons chose between these keys on probe trials. ...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-018-1192-0

    authors: Roberts WA,MacDonald H,Lo KH

    更新日期:2018-07-01 00:00:00

  • Lack of evidence that Tonkean macaques understand what others can hear.

    abstract::By distinguishing the attentional cues of their mates, animals can learn what part of their environment is of potential interest. However, recognizing the attentional states of others through auditory perception appears to be difficult, since these states are generally not accompanied by ostensive signals liable to re...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0795-3

    authors: Costes-Thiré M,Levé M,Uhlrich P,De Marco A,Thierry B

    更新日期:2015-01-01 00:00:00

  • Picture recognition of food by macaques (Macaca silenus).

    abstract::Pictorial representations of three-dimensional objects are often used to investigate animal cognitive abilities; however, investigators rarely evaluate whether the animals conceptualize the two-dimensional image as the object it is intended to represent. We tested for picture recognition in lion-tailed macaques by pre...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-011-0455-9

    authors: Judge PG,Kurdziel LB,Wright RM,Bohrman JA

    更新日期:2012-05-01 00:00:00

  • Quantity matching by an orangutan (Pongo abelii).

    abstract::An adult male orangutan (Pongo abelii) was presented with a series of delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS) tasks in which he was to match images based on (a) the number of individual animals depicted in the photograph (from 1 to 4), (b) the number of abstract shapes presented in the stimulus (from 1 to 4), or (c) the num...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0662-7

    authors: Vonk J

    更新日期:2014-03-01 00:00:00

  • Linear numerosity illusions in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella), rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), and humans (Homo sapiens).

    abstract::Numerosity illusions emerge when the stimuli in one set are overestimated or underestimated relative to the number (or quantity) of stimuli in another set. In the case of multi-item arrays, individual items that form a better Gestalt are more readily grouped, leading to overestimation by human adults and children. As ...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-019-01288-9

    authors: Parrish AE,Beran MJ,Agrillo C

    更新日期:2019-09-01 00:00:00

  • You talkin' to me? An assessment of commands as play signals during dog-human play.

    abstract::During play with a dog, humans commonly command the dog to engage in particular activities. How effective are commands during play, and do they serve as play signals? To answer this question, I examined commands issued to dogs by 21 familiar and 17 unfamiliar persons who played with a dog, and the dog's responses. Spe...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-021-01469-5

    authors: Mitchell RW

    更新日期:2021-01-20 00:00:00

  • Dogs wait longer for better rewards than wolves in a delay of gratification task: but why?

    abstract::Self-control has been shown to be linked with being cooperative and successful in humans and with the g-factor in chimpanzees. As such, it is likely to play an important role in all forms of problem-solving. Self-control, however, does not just vary across individuals but seems also to be dependent on the ecological n...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-020-01346-7

    authors: Range F,Brucks D,Virányi Z

    更新日期:2020-05-01 00:00:00

  • Effects of different training histories upon manufacturing a tool to solve a problem: insight in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.).

    abstract::The emergence of novel behavior is a multilayered phenomenon that comprehends distinct processes. One such process is known as insightful problem solving. "Insight" can be explained as the spontaneous interconnection of previously acquired behavioral repertoires. The objective of this study was to investigate: (1) whe...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-016-1022-1

    authors: Neves Filho HB,de Carvalho Neto MB,Taytelbaum GP,Malheiros RD,Knaus YC

    更新日期:2016-11-01 00:00:00

  • Global and local spatial landmarks: their role during foraging by Columbian ground squirrels (Spermophilus columbianus).

    abstract::Locating food and refuge is essential for an animal's survival. However, little is known how mammals navigate under natural conditions and cope with given environmental constraints. In a series of six experiments, I investigated landmark-based navigation in free-ranging Columbian ground squirrels (Spermophilus columbi...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-005-0006-3

    authors: Vlasak AN

    更新日期:2006-01-01 00:00:00

  • Attentional biases and memory for emotional stimuli in men and male rhesus monkeys.

    abstract::We examined attentional biases for social and non-social emotional stimuli in young adult men and compared the results to those of male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) previously tested in a similar dot-probe task (King et al. in Psychoneuroendocrinology 37(3):396-409, 2012). Recognition memory for these stimuli was a...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0618-y

    authors: Lacreuse A,Schatz K,Strazzullo S,King HM,Ready R

    更新日期:2013-11-01 00:00:00

  • The value of the Piagetian framework for comparative cognitive studies.

    abstract::Although the Piagetian framework has been used by numerous researchers to compare cognitive abilities of diverse species, the system is often criticized as implemented. I examine the various criticisms, suggest ways in which the system can be improved, and argue for the need for descriptive systems such as the Piageti...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-002-0148-5

    authors: Pepperberg IM

    更新日期:2002-09-01 00:00:00

  • A Capuchin monkey (Cebus apella) recognizes when people do and do not know the location of food.

    abstract::In a previous study, Kuroshima and colleagues demonstrated that capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) learned to discriminate between a "knower" who inspected a box for food, and a "guesser" who did not. The aim of the present study was to specify whether the subjects learned a simple conditional discrimination or a causal ...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0184-9

    authors: Kuroshima H,Fujita K,Adachi I,Iwata K,Fuyuki A

    更新日期:2003-12-01 00:00:00

  • Global bias reliability in dogs (Canis familiaris).

    abstract::Dogs enrolled in a previous study were assessed two years later for reliability of their local/global preference in a discrimination test with the same hierarchical stimuli used in the previous study (Experiment 1) and with a novel stimulus (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, dogs easily re-learned to discriminate the po...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-016-1044-8

    authors: Mongillo P,Pitteri E,Sambugaro P,Carnier P,Marinelli L

    更新日期:2017-03-01 00:00:00

  • Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) succeed in a test of quantity conservation.

    abstract::Nonhuman animals demonstrate a number of impressive quantitative skills such as counting sets of items, comparing sets on the basis of the number of items or amount of material, and even responding to simple arithmetic manipulations. In this experiment, capuchin monkeys were presented with a computerized task designed...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0094-3

    authors: Beran MJ

    更新日期:2008-01-01 00:00:00

  • Rule learning by zebra finches in an artificial grammar learning task: which rule?

    abstract::A hallmark of the human language faculty is the use of syntactic rules. The natural vocalizations of animals are syntactically simple, but several studies indicate that animals can detect and discriminate more complex structures in acoustic stimuli. However, how they discriminate such structures is often not clear. Us...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0559-x

    authors: van Heijningen CA,Chen J,van Laatum I,van der Hulst B,ten Cate C

    更新日期:2013-03-01 00:00:00

  • Food and token quantity discrimination in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).

    abstract::Quantity discrimination is adaptive in a variety of ecological contexts and different taxa discriminate stimuli differing in numerousness, both in the wild and in laboratory settings. Quantity discrimination between object arrays has been suggested to be more demanding than between food arrays but, to our knowledge, t...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0111-6

    authors: Addessi E,Crescimbene L,Visalberghi E

    更新日期:2008-04-01 00:00:00

  • How primates acquire their gestures: evaluating current theories and evidence.

    abstract::Mechanisms underlying gesture acquisition in primates are largely unstudied, yet heavily debated. While some studies suggest that gestural repertoires are largely innate, others emphasize that gestures emerge and are shaped in social interactions with other conspecifics. There is agreement, however, regarding the negl...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章,评审

    doi:10.1007/s10071-018-1187-x

    authors: Liebal K,Schneider C,Errson-Lembeck M

    更新日期:2019-07-01 00:00:00

  • Eye as a key element of conspecific image eliciting lateralized response in fish.

    abstract::Visual lateralization in different aspects of social behaviour has been found for numerous species of vertebrates ranging from fish to mammals. For inspection of a shoal mate, many fishes show a left eye-right hemisphere preference. Here, we tested the hypothesis that in fish, there is a key cue in the conspecific app...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0572-0

    authors: Karenina KA,Giljov AN,Malashichev YB

    更新日期:2013-03-01 00:00:00

  • Spontaneous use of tools as straws in great apes.

    abstract::Great apes can use multiple tools to extract food embedded in substrates and can invent new ways to exploit those resources. We tested five bonobos, five chimpanzees, and six orangutans in a task in which they had to use (and modify) a tool as a straw to drink the juice located inside a container. Experiment 1 showed ...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-010-0355-4

    authors: Manrique HM,Call J

    更新日期:2011-03-01 00:00:00

  • Revisiting social recognition systems in invertebrates.

    abstract::Since the 1970s, the ability of some invertebrate species to recognize individual conspecifics has attracted increased scientific interest. However, there is still confusion in the literature, possibly due to the lack of unambiguous criteria for classifying social recognition in its different forms. Here, we synthesiz...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章,评审

    doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0513-y

    authors: Gherardi F,Aquiloni L,Tricarico E

    更新日期:2012-09-01 00:00:00

  • Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem.

    abstract::The trap-tube problem is used to assess whether an individual is able to foresee the outcome of its actions. To solve the task, an animal must use a tool to push a piece of food out of a tube, which has a trap along its length. An animal may learn to avoid the trap through a rule based on associative processes, e.g. u...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0061-4

    authors: Tebbich S,Seed AM,Emery NJ,Clayton NS

    更新日期:2007-04-01 00:00:00

  • Is acoustic evaluation in a non-primate mammal, the tree shrew, affected by context?

    abstract::Sound categorisation plays a crucial role for processing ecological and social stimuli in a species' natural environment. To explore the discrimination and evaluation of sound stimuli in human babies and nonhuman primates, a reciprocal habituation-dishabituation paradigm has been successfully introduced into auditory ...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-011-0411-8

    authors: Konerding WS,Brunke J,Schehka S,Zimmermann E

    更新日期:2011-11-01 00:00:00

  • Cats match voice and face: cross-modal representation of humans in cats (Felis catus).

    abstract::We examined whether cats have a cross-modal representation of humans, using a cross-modal expectancy violation paradigm originally used with dogs by Adachi et al. (Anim Cogn 10:17-21, 2007). We compared cats living in houses and in cat cafés to assess the potential effect of postnatal experience. Cats were presented w...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-019-01265-2

    authors: Takagi S,Arahori M,Chijiiwa H,Saito A,Kuroshima H,Fujita K

    更新日期:2019-09-01 00:00:00

  • Two strings to choose from: do ravens pull the easier one?

    abstract::There are simple co-occurrences as well as functional relationships between events. One may assume that animals detect and use causation rather than mere co-variation. However, understanding causation often requires concepts of hidden forces. In string pulling, obstacles may hamper the access to food. Here, I studied ...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0483-0

    authors: Pfuhl G

    更新日期:2012-07-01 00:00:00

  • Representing tools: how two non-human primate species distinguish between the functionally relevant and irrelevant features of a tool.

    abstract::Few studies have examined whether non-human tool-users understand the properties that are relevant for a tool's function. We tested cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) on an expectancy violation procedure designed to assess whether these species make distinctions between the fun...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0171-1

    authors: Santos LR,Miller CT,Hauser MD

    更新日期:2003-12-01 00:00:00

  • Evidence for discrimination between feeding sounds of familiar fish and unfamiliar mammal-eating killer whale ecotypes by long-finned pilot whales.

    abstract::Killer whales (KW) may be predators or competitors of other cetaceans. Since their foraging behavior and acoustics differ among populations ('ecotypes'), we hypothesized that other cetaceans can eavesdrop on KW sounds and adjust their behavior according to the KW ecotype. We performed playback experiments on long-finn...

    journal_title:Animal cognition

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1007/s10071-019-01282-1

    authors: Curé C,Isojunno S,I Vester H,Visser F,Oudejans M,Biassoni N,Massenet M,Barluet de Beauchesne L,J Wensveen P,Sivle LD,Tyack PL,Miller PJO

    更新日期:2019-09-01 00:00:00