Short-term global motion adaptation induces a compression in the subjective duration of dynamic visual events.

Abstract:

:Apparent duration can be manipulated in a local region of visual field by long-term adaptation to motion or flicker (Johnston, Arnold, & Nishida, 2006). These effects show narrow spatial tuning (Ayhan, Bruno, Nishida, & Johnston, 2009), as well as retinotopic position dependency (Bruno, Ayhan, & Johnston, 2010), supporting early locus in the visual pathway. Here, we introduce a new effect using RDK as a short-term visual adaptor and demonstrate that a brief, subsecond range adaptation induces a significant subjective duration compression (∼10%) on a subsequently presented test stimulus (RDK pattern) only for global motion patterns drifting at 50% motion coherence but not for those drifting at 0% coherence, suggesting a higher level area as a source of origin. In another set of experiments using a plaid stimulus as the adaptor and gratings as the tests, we report again a significant duration compression following a brief motion adaptation, although the effect does not seem to be consistently selective for a particular direction of the standard test relative to that of the plaid adaptor (two-dimensional motion) or its components (one-dimensional motion). Finally, we conduct an experiment using shutter glasses and find that the effects of a short-term adaptor presented monocularly to one eye transfer to the nonadapted eye, providing evidence for the interocular transfer. In a series of control experiments, we also show that the duration effects cannot be explained by adaptation-induced changes in perceived speed, perceived onset-and-offset, and attentional resource allocation. Overall, the duration compression effect requiring motion coherence in RDK, persisting in plaid stimulus, and showing interocular transfer imply explicit genuine mechanisms mediating duration effects in the higher level motion areas.

journal_name

J Vis

journal_title

Journal of vision

authors

Gulhan D,Ayhan I

doi

10.1167/19.5.19

subject

Has Abstract

pub_date

2019-05-01 00:00:00

pages

19

issue

5

issn

1534-7362

pii

2734584

journal_volume

19

pub_type

杂志文章
  • Unseen complex motion is modulated by attention and generates a visible aftereffect.

    abstract::The relationship between attention and awareness and the processing of visual information outside of attention and awareness remain controversial issues. We employed the motion aftereffect (MAE) illusion and continuous flash suppression (CFS) to study the behavioral effects of unseen and unattended visual motion. The ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/11.13.10

    authors: Kaunitz L,Fracasso A,Melcher D

    更新日期:2011-11-09 00:00:00

  • Cross-orientation masking is speed invariant between ocular pathways but speed dependent within them.

    abstract::In human (D. H. Baker, T. S. Meese, & R. J. Summers, 2007b) and in cat (B. Li, M. R. Peterson, J. K. Thompson, T. Duong, & R. D. Freeman, 2005; F. Sengpiel & V. Vorobyov, 2005) there are at least two routes to cross-orientation suppression (XOS): a broadband, non-adaptable, monocular (within-eye) pathway and a more na...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.5.2

    authors: Meese TS,Baker DH

    更新日期:2009-05-04 00:00:00

  • A neurophysiologically plausible population code model for human contrast discrimination.

    abstract::The pedestal effect is the improvement in the detectability of a sinusoidal grating in the presence of another grating of the same orientation, spatial frequency, and phase-usually called the pedestal. Recent evidence has demonstrated that the pedestal effect is differently modified by spectrally flat and notch-filter...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.7.15

    authors: Goris RL,Wichmann FA,Henning GB

    更新日期:2009-07-31 00:00:00

  • Origin of the fast negative ERG component from isolated aspartate-treated mouse retina.

    abstract::The leading edge of the a-wave of the ERG is generally believed to accurately reflect the changes in the circulating current through the cGMP-gated channels in the outer segment plasma membrane of rods and cones. The aspartate-isolated mammalian electroretinogram (ERG) to a rod-saturating flash contains a fast "nose"-...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.12.9

    authors: Vinberg FJ,Strandman S,Koskelainen A

    更新日期:2009-11-17 00:00:00

  • Learning diagnostic features: the delta rule does Bubbles.

    abstract::It has been shown (Murray & Gold, 2004a) that the Bubbles paradigm for studying human perceptual identification can be formally analyzed and compared to reverse correlation methods when the underlying identification model is conceived as a linear amplifier (LAM). However the usefulness of a LAM for characterizing huma...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/13.8.17

    authors: Hannagan T,Grainger J

    更新日期:2013-07-17 00:00:00

  • MRI study of the changes in crystalline lens shape with accommodation and aging in humans.

    abstract::Magnetic Resonance Imaging was used to study changes in the crystalline lens and ciliary body with accommodation and aging. Monocular images were obtained in 15 young (19-29 years) and 15 older (60-70 years) emmetropes when viewing at far (6 m) and at individual near points (14.5 to 20.9 cm) in the younger group. With...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/11.3.19

    authors: Kasthurirangan S,Markwell EL,Atchison DA,Pope JM

    更新日期:2011-03-25 00:00:00

  • Probability and the changing shape of response distributions for orientation.

    abstract::Spatial attention and feature-based attention are regarded as two independent mechanisms for biasing the processing of sensory stimuli. Feature attention is held to be a spatially invariant mechanism that advantages a single feature per sensory dimension. In contrast to the prediction of location independence, I found...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.13.15

    authors: Anderson B

    更新日期:2014-11-18 00:00:00

  • Spatial and temporal limits of motion perception across variations in speed, eccentricity, and low vision.

    abstract::We evaluated spatial displacement and temporal duration thresholds for discriminating the motion direction of gratings for a broad range of speeds (0.06 degrees/s to 30 degrees/s) in fovea and at +/-30 degrees eccentricity. In general, increased speed yielded lower duration thresholds but higher displacement threshold...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/9.1.30

    authors: Lappin JS,Tadin D,Nyquist JB,Corn AL

    更新日期:2009-01-22 00:00:00

  • Discrimination of locomotion direction in impoverished displays of walkers by macaque monkeys.

    abstract::A vast literature exists on human biological motion perception in impoverished displays, e.g., point-light walkers. Less is known about the perception of impoverished biological motion displays in macaques. We trained 3 macaques in the discrimination of facing direction (left versus right) and forward versus backward ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.4.22

    authors: Vangeneugden J,Vancleef K,Jaeggli T,VanGool L,Vogels R

    更新日期:2009-04-28 00:00:00

  • Anatomical constraints on attention: hemifield independence is a signature of multifocal spatial selection.

    abstract::Previous studies have shown independent attentional selection of targets in the left and right visual hemifields during attentional tracking (Alvarez & Cavanagh, 2005) but not during a visual search (Luck, Hillyard, Mangun, & Gazzaniga, 1989). Here we tested whether multifocal spatial attention is the critical process...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/12.5.9

    authors: Alvarez GA,Gill J,Cavanagh P

    更新日期:2012-05-25 00:00:00

  • Anisotropies in judging the direction of moving natural scenes.

    abstract::Although visual systems are optimized to deal with the natural visual environment, our understanding of human motion perception is in large part based on the use of artificial stimuli. Here, we assessed observers' ability to estimate the direction of translating natural images and fractals by having them adjust the or...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.11.5

    authors: Dakin SC,Apthorp D,Alais D

    更新日期:2010-09-03 00:00:00

  • Apparent motion distorts the shape of a stimulus briefly presented along the motion path.

    abstract::We examined whether motion blur accompanying apparent motion (AM) affects the shape of a stimulus presented in the motion path. In a two-alternative forced-choice procedure, observers judged the shape of a Gaussian test stimulus flashed in the path of motion, relative to a reference stimulus, which was a circular Gaus...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.13.15

    authors: Khuu SK,Phu J,Khambiye S

    更新日期:2010-11-24 00:00:00

  • Position shifts following crowded second-order motion adaptation reveal processing of local and global motion without awareness.

    abstract::Adaptation to first-order (luminance defined) motion produces not only a motion aftereffect but also a position aftereffect, in which a target pattern's perceived location is shifted opposite the direction of adaptation. These aftereffects can occur passively (when the direction of motion adaptation cannot be detected...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/7.2.15

    authors: Harp TD,Bressler DW,Whitney D

    更新日期:2007-07-20 00:00:00

  • Mach bands and multiscale models of spatial vision: the role of first, second, and third derivative operators in encoding bars and edges.

    abstract::Ernst Mach observed that light or dark bands could be seen at abrupt changes of luminance gradient in the absence of peaks or troughs in luminance. Many models of feature detection share the idea that bars, lines, and Mach bands are found at peaks and troughs in the output of even-symmetric spatial filters. Our experi...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/12.13.18

    authors: Wallis SA,Georgeson MA

    更新日期:2012-12-21 00:00:00

  • Systematic distortions of perceived planar surface motion in active vision.

    abstract::Recent studies suggest that the active observer combines optic flow information with extra-retinal signals resulting from head motion. Such a combination allows, in principle, a correct discrimination of the presence or absence of surface rotation. In Experiments 1 and 2, observers were asked to perform such discrimin...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.5.12

    authors: Fantoni C,Caudek C,Domini F

    更新日期:2010-05-01 00:00:00

  • We make predictions about eye of origin of visual input: Visual mismatch negativity from binocular rivalry.

    abstract::The visual mismatch negativity (vMMN) is a negative component of event-related potentials (ERPs). It occurs when an infrequent visual stimulus, a deviant, is randomly and unpredictably presented in a sequence of frequent visual stimuli, the standards, and is thought to reflect prediction and prediction error of visual...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.13.9

    authors: Jack BN,Roeber U,O'Shea RP

    更新日期:2015-01-01 00:00:00

  • Suprathreshold contrast summation over area using drifting gratings.

    abstract::This study investigated contrast summation over area for moving targets applied to a fixed-size contrast pedestal-a technique originally developed by Meese and Summers (2007) to demonstrate strong spatial summation of contrast for static patterns at suprathreshold contrast levels. Target contrast increments (drifting ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/18.4.20

    authors: McDougall TJ,Dickinson JE,Badcock DR

    更新日期:2018-04-01 00:00:00

  • A moving-barber-pole illusion.

    abstract::In the barber-pole illusion (BPI), a diagonally moving grating is perceived as moving vertically because of the shape of the vertically oriented window through which it is viewed-a strong shape-motion interaction. We introduce a novel stimulus-the moving barber pole-in which a diagonal, drifting sinusoidal carrier is ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/14.5.1

    authors: Sun P,Chubb C,Sperling G

    更新日期:2014-05-01 00:00:00

  • Pursuit of the ineffable: perceptual and motor reversals during the tracking of apparent motion.

    abstract::Pursuit can be guided by perceived rather than physical motion, but the temporal relationship between motion perception and pursuit is unknown. We used an apparent motion stimulus consisting of a horizontal row of evenly spaced Kanizsa illusory squares (1.44 deg2): the illusory contours appeared at the midpoints of th...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/3.11.1

    authors: Madelain L,Krauzlis RJ

    更新日期:2003-11-18 00:00:00

  • Visual motion, eye motion, and relative motion: A parametric fMRI study of functional specializations of smooth pursuit eye movement network areas.

    abstract::The ability to pursue moving objects with the eyes is vital to humans. However, it remains unclear how the brain differentiates visual object motion, smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM), and eye movement-induced relative motion on the retina and where visual-to-oculomotor transformation takes place. To characterize fu...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.14.21

    authors: Ohlendorf S,Sprenger A,Speck O,Glauche V,Haller S,Kimmig H

    更新日期:2010-12-20 00:00:00

  • Fourth-root summation of contrast over area: No end in sight when spatially inhomogeneous sensitivity is compensated by a witch's hat.

    abstract::Measurements of area summation for luminance-modulated stimuli are typically confounded by variations in sensitivity across the retina. Recently we conducted a detailed analysis of sensitivity across the visual field (Baldwin, Meese, & Baker, 2012) and found it to be well described by a bilinear "witch's hat" function...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.15.4

    authors: Baldwin AS,Meese TS

    更新日期:2015-01-01 00:00:00

  • Dichoptic color saturation mixture: Binocular luminance contrast promotes perceptual averaging.

    abstract::We demonstrate a new type of interaction between suprathreshold color (chromatic) and luminance contrast in the context of binocular vision. When two isoluminant colored disks of identical hue but different saturations are presented to different eyes, the apparent saturation of the resulting "dichoptic" mix is close t...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.5.2

    authors: Kingdom FA,Libenson L

    更新日期:2015-01-01 00:00:00

  • Robust object-based encoding in visual working memory.

    abstract::Recently, researchers have begun to investigate how nonspatial perceptual information is extracted into visual working memory (VWM), focusing particularly on object-based encoding (OBE). That is, whenever even one feature-dimension is selected for entry into VWM, the others are also extracted automatically. While ther...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/13.2.1

    authors: Shen M,Tang N,Wu F,Shui R,Gao Z

    更新日期:2013-02-01 00:00:00

  • Larger visual stimuli are perceived to last longer from time to time: The internal clock is not affected by nontemporal visual stimulus size.

    abstract::Performance on interval timing is often explained by the assumption of an internal clock based on neural counting. According to this account, a neural pacemaker generates pulses, and the number of pulses relating to a physical time interval is recorded by a counter. Thus, the number of accumulated pulses is the intern...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/15.3.5

    authors: Rammsayer TH,Verner M

    更新日期:2015-03-10 00:00:00

  • Slow feature analysis yields a rich repertoire of complex cell properties.

    abstract::In this study we investigate temporal slowness as a learning principle for receptive fields using slow feature analysis, a new algorithm to determine functions that extract slowly varying signals from the input data. We find a good qualitative and quantitative match between the set of learned functions trained on imag...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/5.6.9

    authors: Berkes P,Wiskott L

    更新日期:2005-07-20 00:00:00

  • Reaching and grasping actions and their context shape the perception of object size.

    abstract::Humans frequently estimate the size of objects to grasp them. In fact, when performing an action, our perception is focused towards the visual properties of the object that enable us to successfully execute the action. However, the motor system is also able to influence perception, but only a few studies have reported...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/17.12.10

    authors: Bosco A,Daniele F,Fattori P

    更新日期:2017-10-01 00:00:00

  • Differential changes in human perception of speed due to motion adaptation.

    abstract::Visual systems adapt to the prevailing image conditions. This improves the ability to discriminate between two similar stimuli but has the side effect that veridical perception is degraded. For example, prolonged driving at 100 km/h may reduce the perceived speed to 80 km/h but improve the sensitivity to changes in th...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.11.6

    authors: Hietanen MA,Crowder NA,Ibbotson MR

    更新日期:2008-08-11 00:00:00

  • Where do we store the memory representations that guide attention?

    abstract::During the last decade one of the most contentious and heavily studied topics in the attention literature has been the role that working memory representations play in controlling perceptual selection. The hypothesis has been advanced that to have attention select a certain perceptual input from the environment, we on...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章,评审

    doi:10.1167/13.3.1

    authors: Woodman GF,Carlisle NB,Reinhart RM

    更新日期:2013-02-25 00:00:00

  • Eye movement statistics in humans are consistent with an optimal search strategy.

    abstract::Most models of visual search are based on the intuition that humans choose fixation locations containing features that best match the features of the target. The optimal version of this feature-based strategy is what we term "maximum a posteriori (MAP) search." Alternatively, humans could choose fixations that maximiz...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/8.3.4

    authors: Najemnik J,Geisler WS

    更新日期:2008-03-07 00:00:00

  • What is second-order vision for? Discriminating illumination versus material changes.

    abstract::The human visual system is sensitive to second-order modulations of the local contrast (CM) or amplitude (AM) of a carrier signal. Second-order cues are detected independently of first-order luminance signals; however, it is not clear why vision should benefit from second-order sensitivity. Analysis of the first- and ...

    journal_title:Journal of vision

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1167/10.9.2

    authors: Schofield AJ,Rock PB,Sun P,Jiang X,Georgeson MA

    更新日期:2010-07-16 00:00:00