Choice with frequently changing food rates and food ratios.

Abstract:

:In studies of operant choice, when one schedule of a concurrent pair is varied while the other is held constant, the constancy of the constant schedule may exert discriminative control over performance. In our earlier experiments, schedules varied reciprocally across components within sessions, so that while food ratio varied food rate remained constant. In the present experiment, we held one variable-interval (VI) schedule constant while varying the concurrent VI schedule within sessions. We studied five conditions, each with a different constant left VI schedule. On the right key, seven different VI schedules were presented in seven different unsignaled components. We analyzed performances at several different time scales. At the longest time scale, across conditions, behavior ratios varied with food ratios as would be expected from the generalized matching law. At shorter time scales, effects due to holding the left VI constant became more and more apparent, the shorter the time scale. In choice relations across components, preference for the left key leveled off as the right key became leaner. Interfood choice approximated strict matching for the varied right key, whereas interfood choice hardly varied at all for the constant left key. At the shortest time scale, visit patterns differed for the left and right keys. Much evidence indicated the development of a fix-and-sample pattern. In sum, the procedural difference made a large difference to performance, except for choice at the longest time scale and the fix-and-sample pattern at the shortest time scale.

journal_name

J Exp Anal Behav

authors

Baum WM,Davison M

doi

10.1002/jeab.70

subject

Has Abstract

pub_date

2014-03-01 00:00:00

pages

246-74

issue

2

eissn

0022-5002

issn

1938-3711

journal_volume

101

pub_type

杂志文章
  • Notes on discounting.

    abstract::In general, if a variable can be expressed as a function of its own maximum value, that function may be called a discount function. Delay discounting and probability discounting are commonly studied in psychology, but memory, matching, and economic utility also may be viewed as discounting processes. When they are so ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.2006.85-05

    authors: Rachlin H

    更新日期:2006-05-01 00:00:00

  • Drug discrimination under a concurrent schedule.

    abstract::Three pigeons were trained to discriminate a 5.0 mg/kg dose of pentobarbital from saline under a two-key concurrent schedule with responding on the key associated with the presession injection, under both stimulus conditions, producing four times as many reinforcers as responding on the other key. This concurrent sche...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1996.65-495

    authors: Snodgrass SH,McMillan DE

    更新日期:1996-05-01 00:00:00

  • The generality of selective observing.

    abstract::Four rats obtained food pellets by poking a key and 5-s presentations of the discriminative stimuli by pressing a lever. Every 1 or 2 min, the prevailing schedule of reinforcement for key poking alternated between rich (either variable-interval [VI] 30 s or VI 60 s) and lean (either VI 240 s, VI 480 s, or extinction) ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.2002.77-171

    authors: Gaynor ST,Shull RL

    更新日期:2002-03-01 00:00:00

  • Comparisons between one-key and two-key versions of the sinewave schedule for pigeons.

    abstract::When the rate of reinforcement for pigeons' key pecking varied over time following a sine waveform, performances were more consistent and reliable if a constant-rate reinforcement schedule was concurrently available on a second key than if only the sinewave-varying reinforcement schedule was available. In the two-key ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1982.38-101

    authors: Johnson DF,Wheeler HP

    更新日期:1982-07-01 00:00:00

  • The magnitude-of-reinforcement function in closed and open economies.

    abstract::It has been hypothesized that the magnitude-of-reinforcement effect may differ in closed and open experimental economies. We determined the relationship between magnitude of reinforcement and response rate in three feeding conditions: a closed economy in which total intake was unrestricted, a closed economy in which t...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 临床试验,杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1992.57-81

    authors: Collier G,Johnson DF,Morgan C

    更新日期:1992-01-01 00:00:00

  • The effect of reinforcer delays on the form of the forgetting function.

    abstract::Pigeons were trained in a matching-to-sample procedure with retention intervals of 0, 2, 4, 6, and 8 s mixed within each session. In different conditions, reinforcement was delayed by 0, 1, 2, 4, 6, or 8 s from correct choice responses. Discriminability decreased with increasing retention-interval duration and with in...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.2003.80-77

    authors: Sargisson RJ,White KG

    更新日期:2003-07-01 00:00:00

  • Behavioral control by an imprinted stimulus: long-term effects.

    abstract::Newly hatched ducklings were exposed to imprinting procedures and subsequently trained to peck a key by presenting the imprinting stimulus as the reinforcing (response-contingent) event. Individual ducklings then lived in the apparatus under an arrangement in which each peck produced a 15-sec stimulus presentation. Fo...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1967.10-495

    authors: Hoffman HS,Kozma F Jr

    更新日期:1967-09-01 00:00:00

  • Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency.

    abstract::Where do equivalence relations come from? One possible answer is that they arise directly from the reinforcement contingency. That is to say, a reinforcement contingency produces two types of outcome: (a) 2-, 3-, 4-, 5-, or n-term units of analysis that are known, respectively, as operant reinforcement, simple discrim...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.2000.74-127

    authors: Sidman M

    更新日期:2000-07-01 00:00:00

  • Accuracy of discrimination, rate of responding, and resistance to change.

    abstract::Pigeons were trained on multiple schedules in which responding on a center key produced matching-to-sample trials according to the same variable-interval 30-s schedules in both components. Matching trials consisted of a vertical or tilted line sample on the center key followed by vertical and tilted comparisons on the...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.2003.79-307

    authors: Nevin JA,Milo J,Odum AL,Shahan TA

    更新日期:2003-05-01 00:00:00

  • Interlocking schedules of reinforcement.

    abstract::Four male pigmented rats were exposed to a procedure designed to investigate the relation between several performance measures and a schedule continuum ranging from FR 36 to FI 2 through four intermediate interlocking schedules. On all schedules, each subject developed a stable performance that was generally break-and...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1962.5-213

    authors: BERRYMAN R,NEVIN JA

    更新日期:1962-04-01 00:00:00

  • Delay discounting in Lewis and Fischer 344 rats: steady-state and rapid-determination adjusting-amount procedures.

    abstract::Lewis rats have been shown to make more impulsive choices than Fischer 344 rats in discrete-trial choice procedures that arrange fixed (i.e., nontitrating) reinforcement parameters. However, nontitrating procedures yield only gross estimates of preference, as choice measures in animal subjects are rarely graded at the...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.2012.97-305

    authors: Stein JS,Pinkston JW,Brewer AT,Francisco MT,Madden GJ

    更新日期:2012-05-01 00:00:00

  • A comparison of two types of extinction following fixed-ratio training.

    abstract::Five groups of pigeons were food reinforced on various schedules. Half of each group were extinguished in the normal manner; the others were presented with a stimulus change, previously paired with reinforcement, each time they completed their respective fixed ratios. Response rate in training was an increasing negati...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1966.9-41

    authors: Weissman NW,Crossman EK

    更新日期:1966-01-01 00:00:00

  • Temporal framing and the hidden-zero effect: rate-dependent outcomes on delay discounting.

    abstract::Recent research suggests that presenting time intervals as units (e.g., days) or as specific dates, can modulate the degree to which humans discount delayed outcomes. Another framing effect involves explicitly stating that choosing a smaller-sooner reward is mutually exclusive to receiving a larger-later reward, thus ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1002/jeab.328

    authors: Naudé GP,Kaplan BA,Reed DD,Henley AJ,DiGennaro Reed FD

    更新日期:2018-05-01 00:00:00

  • Analysis of fixed-ratio behavior maintained by drug reinforcers.

    abstract::Behavior maintained by intravenously delivered alfentanil, cocaine, or ketamine was assessed using a fixed-ratio schedule of reinforcement. As the dose of each drug was increased, rate of responding also increased up to a maximum. Further increases in dose resulted in decreased response rates (inverted U-shaped curve)...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1991.56-331

    authors: Skjoldager P,Winger G,Woods JH

    更新日期:1991-09-01 00:00:00

  • Two parameters of a temporally defined schedule of negative reinforcement.

    abstract::Temporal schedules of negative reinforcement were derived in a manner analogous to those for positive reinforcement, and two parametric studies were carried out with two of the three defining variables. With some limiting values of the defining variables, escape schedules result and data obtained at these values were ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1963.6-361

    authors: SIDLEY NA

    更新日期:1963-07-01 00:00:00

  • Concurrent fixed-interval variable-ratio schedules and the matching relation.

    abstract::Five rats responded under concurrent fixed-interval variable-ratio schedules of food reinforcement. Fixed-interval values ranged from 50-seconds to 300-seconds and variable-ratio values ranged from 30 to 360; a five-second changeover delay was in effect throughout the experiment. The relations between reinforcement ra...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1981.36-317

    authors: Rider DP

    更新日期:1981-11-01 00:00:00

  • Classical avoidance without a warning stimulus.

    abstract::White rats were scheduled to be shocked every 15 sec; but they were given a limited time interval between shocks when they could prevent the next scheduled shock from occurring if they pressed a lever. The duration of this limited avoidance period was varied, as was its location within the interval between scheduled s...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1962.5-97

    authors: SIDMAN M

    更新日期:1962-01-01 00:00:00

  • Base-rate neglect as a function of base rates in probabilistic contingency learning.

    abstract::When humans predict criterion events based on probabilistic predictors, they often lend excessive weight to the predictor and insufficient weight to the base rate of the criterion event. In an operant analysis, using a matching-to-sample paradigm, Goodie and Fantino (1996) showed that humans exhibit base-rate neglect ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.2008.90-23

    authors: Kutzner F,Freytag P,Vogel T,Fiedler K

    更新日期:2008-07-01 00:00:00

  • Sample-duration effects on pigeons' delayed matching as a function of predictability of duration.

    abstract::Three experiments assessed the impact of sample duration on pigeons' delayed matching as a function of whether or not the samples themselves signaled how long they would remain on. When duration was uncorrelated with the sample appearing on each matching trial, the typical effect of duration was observed: Choice accur...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1999.72-279

    authors: Urcuioli PJ,DeMarse TB,Lionello KM

    更新日期:1999-11-01 00:00:00

  • A multivariate rate equation for variable-interval performance.

    abstract::A value-like parameter is introduced into a rate equation for describing variable-interval performance. The equation, derived solely from formal considerations, expresses rate of responding as a joint function of rate of reinforcement and "reinforcer power." Preliminary tests of the rate equation show that it handles ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1979.31-267

    authors: McDowell JJ,Kessel R

    更新日期:1979-03-01 00:00:00

  • Implicit sequence learning in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta).

    abstract::Implicit learning involves picking up information from the environment without explicit instruction or conscious awareness of the learning process. In nonhuman animals, conscious awareness is impossible to assess, so we define implicit learning as occurring when animals acquire information beyond what is required for ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1002/jeab.180

    authors: Drucker CB,Baghdoyan T,Brannon EM

    更新日期:2016-01-01 00:00:00

  • The recurrence of negatively reinforced responding of humans.

    abstract::The recurrence of negatively reinforced responding of humans was studied in three experiments. In each experiment during Baseline, key-pressing produced 3-s timeouts from a requirement to exert finger pressure on a force cell according to variable- or fixed-ratio schedules of reinforcement. In Experiment 1, resurgence...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1002/jeab.178

    authors: Alessandri J,Lattal KA,Cançado CR

    更新日期:2015-11-01 00:00:00

  • FORCE GRADIENTS IN STIMULUS GENERALIZATION.

    abstract::Pigeons were trained to peck with differing force requirements on a key specially designed to measure and control force of pecking without changes in the mechanical threshold. Following training on a key illuminated by a single wavelength, a generalization test was given. Force and rate gradients were obtained. Force ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1965.8-231

    authors: COLE JL

    更新日期:1965-07-01 00:00:00

  • Choice between response units: The rate constancy model.

    abstract::In a conjoint schedule, reinforcement is available simultaneously on two or more schedules for the same response. The present experiments provided food for key pecking on both a random-interval and a differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) schedule. Experiment 1 involved ordinary DRL schedules; Experiment 2 added...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1983.39-275

    authors: Zeiler MD,Blakely TF

    更新日期:1983-03-01 00:00:00

  • Rapid determinations of preference in multiple concurrent-chain schedules.

    abstract::With concurrent chains arranged for a pigeon's key pecks, pecks on two concurrently available initial-link keys (left and right) respectively produce separately operating terminal links (A and B). Preferences for terminal link A over terminal link B are usually calculated as deviations of relative initial-link respons...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1986.46-211

    authors: Cerutti DT,Catania AC

    更新日期:1986-09-01 00:00:00

  • Contingency discriminability and peak shift in concurrent schedules.

    abstract::We investigated the effects of discriminative stimuli on choice in a highly variable environment using a procedure in which multiple two-key concurrent VI VI components changed every 10 reinforcers and were signaled by differential flashes of red and yellow keylights. Across conditions, five pigeons were exposed to a ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.2006.11-05

    authors: Krägeloh CU,Elliffe DM,Davison M

    更新日期:2006-07-01 00:00:00

  • TEMPORALLY SPACED RESPONDING BY PIGEONS: DEVELOPMENT AND EFFECTS OF DEPRIVATION AND EXTINCTION.

    abstract::In the first five or six sessions on a DRL 20-sec schedule of reinforcement there developed a stable performance characterized by a relatively constant conditional probability of occurrence (IRTs/op) of interresponse times (IRTs) of durations greater than 5 or 6 sec. Extinction and the level of deprivation changed bot...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1964.7-415

    authors: REYNOLDS GS

    更新日期:1964-11-01 00:00:00

  • Blocking, unblocking, and overexpectation in autoshaping with pigeons.

    abstract::Three experiments used pigeons in an autoshaping procedure and a single-subject design to examine compound stimulus control in classical conditioning. Experiment 1 examined the blocking effect, and Experiment 2 examined the unblocking effect. In both experiments, response-independent food was first delivered intermitt...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1996.65-575

    authors: Khallad Y,Moore J

    更新日期:1996-05-01 00:00:00

  • Key pecking in pigeons produced by pairing keylight with inaccessible grain.

    abstract::In Experiment I, keylight was paired with inaccessible grain delivery (under two conditions of keylight intensity) to determine if autoshaping would occur in the absence of primary reinforcement. In Experiment II, the procedure was repeated with accessible grain, for comparison. In Experiment III, the procedures were ...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1975.23-199

    authors: Zentall TR,Hogan DE

    更新日期:1975-03-01 00:00:00

  • Stimulus generalization as a function of the delay between training and testing procedures: a reevaluation.

    abstract::Three groups of 12 pigeons each were trained to discriminate between lights of 550 mmu (S(D)), correlated with 1-min variable-interval reinforcement and 570 mmu (S(Delta)), correlated with extinction. Group A was tested for wavelength generalization in extinction 1 min after meeting the discrimination criterion; Group...

    journal_title:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior

    pub_type: 杂志文章

    doi:10.1901/jeab.1969.12-105

    authors: Thomas DR,Burr DE

    更新日期:1969-01-01 00:00:00